Skip to main content
Original Article

Narrative Media’s Emphasis on Distinct Moral Intuitions Alters Early Adolescents’ Judgments

Published Online:https://doi.org/10.1027/1864-1105/a000307

Abstract. Logic from the model of intuitive morality and exemplars (MIME) suggests that narrative media emphasizing moral intuitions can increase the salience of those intuitions in audiences. To date, support for this logic has been limited to adults. Across two studies, the present research tested MIME predictions in early adolescents (ages 10–14). The salience of care, fairness, loyalty, and authority intuitions was manipulated in a pilot study with verbal prompts (N = 87) and in the main study with a comic book (N = 107). In both studies, intuition salience was measured after induction. The pilot study demonstrated that exposure to verbal prompts emphasizing care, fairness, and loyalty increased the salience of their respective intuitions. The main study showed that exposure to comic books emphasizing all four separate intuitions increased salience of their respective intuitions in early adolescents. Results are discussed in terms of relevance for the MIME and understanding narrative media’s influence on children’s moral judgments.

References

  • Anderson, C. A., Berkowitz, L., Donnerstein, E., Huesmann, L. R., Johnson, J. D., Linz, D., Malamuth, N. M., & Wartella, E. (2003). The influence of media violence on youth. Psychological Science in the Public Interest, 4(3), 81–110. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1529-1006.2003.pspi_1433.x First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Bandura, A. (1989). Human agency in social cognitive theory. American Psychologist, 44, 1175–1184. https://doi.org/10.1037/0003-066X.44.9.1175 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Buckley, K. E., & Anderson, C. A. (2006). A theoretical model of the effects and consequences of playing video games. In P. VordererJ. BryantEds., Playing video games: Motives, responses, and consequences (pp. 363–378). Lawrence Erlbaum Associates. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Cashdan, E. (1998). Adaptiveness of food learning and food aversions in children. Social Science Information, 37, 613–632. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Champely, S., Ekstrom, C., Dalgaard, P., Gill, J., Weibelzahl, S., Anandkumar, A., Ford, C., Volcic, R., & De Rosario, H. (2020). pwr. R Package version 1.3-0. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Cho, H. C., & Abe, S. (2013). Is two-tailed testing for directional research hypotheses tests legitimate? Journal of Business Research, 66(9), 1261–1266. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jbusres.2012.02.023 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Cingel, D. P., & Krcmar, M. (2020). Considering moral foundations theory and the model of intuitive morality and exemplars in the context of child and adolescent development. Annals of the International Communication Association, 44(2), 129–138. https://doi.org/10.1080/23808985.2020.1755337 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • de Leeuw, R. N., & van der Laan, C. A. (2018). Helping behavior in Disney animated movies and children’s helping behavior in the Netherlands. Journal of Children and Media, 12, 159–174. https://doi.org/10.1080/17482798.2017.1409245 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Eden, A. L. (2011). The influence of moral behaviors on person perception processes: An fMRI investigation (Doctoral dissertation). Michigan State University. Retrieved from MSU Libraries Electronic Theses and Dissertations. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Eisenberg-Berg, N. (1979). Development of children’s prosocial moral judgment. Developmental Psychology, 15, 128–137. https://doi.org/10.1037/0012-1649.15.2.128 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Fields, A. (2000). Contrasts and post hoc tests for one-way independent ANOVA using SPSS. https://web.archive.org/web/20181221035127/http://www.discoveringstatistics.com/repository/contrasts.pdf First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Fisch, S. (2005). Children’s learning from television. TelevIZIon, 18, 10–14. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Gentile, D. A., Groves, C. L., & Gentile, J. R. (2014). The general learning model: Unveiling the learning potential in video games. In F. BlumbergEd., Learning by playing: Video gaming in education (pp. 121–142). Oxford University Press. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Geraci, A., & Surian, L. (2011). The developmental roots of fairness: Infants’ reactions to equal and unequal distributions of resources. Developmental Science, 14, 1012–1102. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-7687.2011.01048.x First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Graham, J., Nosek, B. A., Haidt, J., Iyer, R., Koleva, S., & Ditto, P. H. (2011). Mapping the moral domain. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 101, 366–385. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0021847 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Hahn, L., Tamborini, R., Prabhu, S., Klebig, B., Grall, C., & Pei, D. (2017). The importance of altruistic versus egoistic motivations: A content analysis of conflicted motivations in children’s television programming. Communication Reports, 30, 67–79. https://doi.org/10.1080/08934215.2016.1251602 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Haidt, J. (2001). The emotional dog and its rational tail: A social intuitionist approach to moral judgment. Psychological Review, 108, 814–834. https://doi.org/10.1037/0033-295X.108.4.814 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Haidt, J., & Bjorklund, F. (2008). Social intuitionists answer six questions about morality. In W. Sinnott-ArmstrongEd., Moral psychology: Vol. 2. The cognitive science of morality (pp. 181–217). MIT Press. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Haidt, J., & Joseph, C. (2007). The moral mind: How 5 sets of innate intuitions guide the development of many culture-specific virtues, and perhaps even modules. In P. CarruthersS. LaurenceS. StichEds., The innate mind (Vol. 3, pp. 367–391). Oxford University Press. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Hamlin, J. K. (2013). Moral judgment and action in preverbal infants and toddlers evidence for an innate moral core. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 22, 186–193. https://doi.org/10.1177/0963721412470687 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Hamlin, J. K., Mahajan, N., Liberman, Z., & Wynn, K. (2013). Not like me = bad. Infants prefer those who harm dissimilar others. Psychological Science, 24, 589–594. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Huesmann, L., & Taylor, L. D. (2006). The role of media violence in violent behavior. Annual Review of Public Health, 27, 393–415. https://doi.org/10.1146/annurev.publhealth.26.021304.144640 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Huff, M., Rosenfelder, D., Oberbeck, M., Merkt, M., Papenmeier, F., & Meitz, T. G. (2020). Cross-codal integration of bridging-event information in narrative understanding. Memory & Cognition, 48, 942–956. https://doi.org/10.3758/s13421-020-01039-z First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Kohlberg, L. (1981). Essays on moral development, Vol. I: The philosophy of moral development. Harper & Row. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Laupa, M. (1991). Children’s reasoning about three authority attributes: Adult status, knowledge, and social position. Developmental Psychology, 27, 321–329. https://doi.org/10.1037/0012-1649.27.2.321 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Levine, T., & Banas, J. (2002). One-tailed F-tests in communication research. Communication Monographs, 69, 132–143. https://doi.org/10.1080/714041709 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Mares, M. L., & Acosta, E. E. (2008). Be kind to three-legged dogs: Children’s literal interpretations of TV’s moral lessons. Media Psychology, 11, 377–399. https://doi.org/10.1080/15213260802204355 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Mares, M. L., & Braun, M. T. (2013). Effects of conflict in tween sitcoms on US students’ moral reasoning about social exclusion. Journal of Children and Media, 7, 428–445. https://doi.org/10.1080/17482798.2013.785972 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Miller, G. (2008). The roots of morality. Science, 320, 734–737. https://doi.org/10.1126/science.32.5877.734 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Prabhu, S. (2018). Measuring the accessibility of moral intuitions: A validation study of the Moral Foundations-Affect Misattribution Procedure (MF-AMP) (Doctoral Dissertation). Michigan State University. https://d.lib.msu.edu/etd/19422 First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Roskos-Ewoldsen, D. R., Roskos-Ewoldsen, B., & Carpentier, F. D. (2009). Media priming: An updated synthesis. In J. BryantM. B. OliverEds., Media effects: Advances in theory and research (pp. 74–93). Routledge. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Sagi, A., & Hoffman, M. L. (1976). Empathic distress in the newborn. Developmental Psychology, 12, 175–176. https://doi.org/10.1037/0012-1649.12.2.175 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Tamborini, R. (2013). Model of intuitive morality and exemplars. In R. TamboriniEd., Media and the moral mind (pp. 43–74). Routledge. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Tamborini, R., Hofer, M., Prabhu, S., Grall, C., Novotny, E., Hahn, L., & Klebig, B. (2017). The impact of terror attack news on moral intuitions and moral behavior towards outgroups. Mass Communication and Society, 20, 800–824. https://doi.org/10.1080/15205436.2017.1342130 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Tamborini, R., Lewis, R. L., Prabhu, S., Grizzard, M., Hahn, L., & Wang, L. (2016). Media’s influence on the accessibility of altruistic and egoistic motivations. Communication Research Reports, 33, 177–187. https://doi.org/10.1080/08824096.2016.1186627 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Tamborini, R., Prabhu, S., Lewis, R. J., Grizzard, M., & Eden, A. (2016). The influence of media exposure on the accessibility of moral intuitions and associated affect. Journal of Media Psychology, 30, 79–90. https://doi.org/10.1027/1864-1105/a000183 First citation in articleLinkGoogle Scholar

  • Turiel, E. (2008). The development of morality. In W. DamonR. M. LernerEds., Handbook of child psychology (pp. 473–516). Wiley. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • van den Broek, P., Lynch, J. S., Naslund, J., Ievers-Landis, C. E., & Verduin, K. (2003). The development of comprehension of main ideas in narratives: Evidence from the selection of titles. Journal of Educational Psychology, 95, 707–718. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-0663.95.4.707 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Zillmann, D. (2002). Exemplification theory of media influence. In J. BryantD. ZillmanEds., Media effects: Advances in theory and research (2nd ed., pp. 19–41). Lawrence Erlbaum Associates. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar