Skip to main content
Original Article

If I Cry, Do You Care?

Individual Differences in Empathy Moderate the Facilitation of Caregiving Words After Exposure to Crying Faces

Published Online:https://doi.org/10.1027/1614-0001/a000098

Crying is a powerful solicitation of caregiving, yet little is known about the cognitive processes underpinning caring responses to crying others. This study examined (1) whether crying (compared to sad and happy) faces differentially elicited semantic activation of caregiving, and (2) whether individual differences in cognitive and emotional empathy moderated this activation. Ninety participants completed a lexical decision task in which caregiving, neutral, and nonwords were presented after subliminal exposure (24 ms) to crying, sad, and happy faces. Individuals low in cognitive empathy had slower reaction times to caregiving (vs. neutral) words after exposure to crying faces, but not after sad or happy faces. Results are discussed with respect to the role of empathy in response to crying others.

References

  • Aiken, L. S. , West, S. G. (1991). Multiple regression: Testing and interpreting interactions. Newbury Park, CA: Sage. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Balota, D. A. , Yap, M. J. , Cortese, M. J., Hutchison, K. A. , Kessler, B., Loftus, B. , ... Treiman, R. (2007). The English lexicon project. Behavior Research Methods, 39, 445–459. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Baron-Cohen, S. , & Wheelwright, S. (2004). The Empathy Quotient: An investigation of adults with Aspergers Syndrome or High Functioning Autism, and normal sex differences. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 34, 163–175. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Batson, C. D. (1998). Altruism and prosocial behavior. In D. T. Gilbert S. T. Fiske G. LindzeyEds., The handbook of social psychology (pp. 282–316). Boston, MD: McGraw-Hill. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Bieneck, S. , Krahe, B. (2011). Blaming the victim and exonerating the perpetrator in cases of rape and robbery: Is there a double standard? Journal of Interpersonal Violence, 26, 1785–1797. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Blair, R. J. (2005). Responding to the emotions of others: Dissociating forms of empathy through the study of typical and psychiatric populations. Consciousness and Cognition, 14, 698–718. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Campos-Melady, M. , & Smith, J. E. (2012). Memory associations between negative emotions and alcohol on the lexical decision task predict alcohol use in women. Addictive Behavior, 37(1), 60–66. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Cornelius, R. , & Lubliner, E. (2003). The what and why of others’ responses to our tears: Adult crying as an attachment behavior. In Proceedings of the Third International Conference on The (Non)expression of Emotions in Health and Disease. Tilburg, The Netherlands: Tilburg University Press. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • de Vingemont, F. , & Singer, T. (2006). The empathic brain: How, when and why? Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 10, 435–441. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Dovidio, J. F. , Allen, J. L., Schroeder, D. A. , (1990). The specificity of empathy-induced helping: Evidence for altruistic motivation. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 59, 249–260. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Fischler, I. S. , & Bloom, P. A. (1979). Automatic and attentional processes in the effects of sentence contexts on word recognition. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 18, 1–20. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Fussell, N. J. , Rowe, A. C., Mohr, C. (2011). Hemispheric processing of differently-valenced and self-relevant attachment words in middle-aged married and separated individuals. Laterality: Asymmetries of Body, Brain and Cognition, 1, 1–33. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Gery, I. , Miljkovitch, R., Berthoz, S. , Soussignan, R. (2009). Empathy and recognition of facial expressions of emotion in sex offenders, non sex offenders, and normal controls. Psychiatry Research, 165, 252–262. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Hendriks, M. , Croon, M. A., Vingerhoets, A. J. J. M. (2008). Social reactions to adult crying: The help-soliciting function of tears. The Journal of Social Psychology, 148, 22–42. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Hendriks, M. , Vingerhoets, A. J. J. M. (2006). Social messages of crying faces: Their influence on anticipated person perception, emotions and behavioral responses. Cognition and Emotion, 20, 878–886. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Kemeny, M. E. , Foltz, C. , Cavanagh, J. F., Cullen, M. , Giese-Davis, J., Jennings, P. , ... Ekman, P. (2012). Contemplative/emotion training reduces negative emotional behavior and promotes prosocial responses. Emotion, 12, 338–350. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Kottler, J. A. , & Montgomery, M. J. (2001). Theories of crying. In A. J. J. M. Vingerhoets R. CorneliusEds., Adult crying: A biopsychosocial approach (pp. 1–17). Hove, UK: Brunner-Routledge. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Martin, R. A. , Berry, G. E., Dobranski, T., Horne, M. , Dodgson, P. G. (1996). Emotion perception threshold: Individual differences in emotional sensitivity. Journal of Research in Personality, 30, 290–305. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Masten, C. L. , Morelli, S. , Eisenberger, N. I. (2011). An fMRI investigation of empathy for “social pain” and subsequent prosocial behavior. Neuroimage, 55(1), 381–388. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Meyer, D. , Schvaneveldt, R. W. (1971). Facilitation in recognizing pairs of words: Evidence of dependence between retrieval operations. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 90, 227–234. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Mikulincer, M. , Gillath, O., Shaver, P. R. (2002). Activation of the attachment system in adulthood: Threat-related primes increase the accessibility of mental representations of attachment figures. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 83, 881–895. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Muncer, S. , Ling, J. (2006). Psychometric analysis of the empathy quotient (EQ) scale. Personality and Individual Differences, 40, 1111–1119. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Patel, V. (1993). Crying behavior and psychiatric disorder in adults: A review. Comprehensive Psychiatry, 34, 206–211. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Provine, R. , Krosnowski, K. , Brocato, N. (2009). Tearing: Breakthrough in human emotional signaling. Evolutionary Psychology, 7(1), 52–56. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Ratcliff, R. (1993). Methods for dealing with reaction time outliers. Psychological Bulletin, 114, 510–532. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Rowe, A. C. , Carnelley, K. B. (2003). Attachment style differences in the processing of attachment relevant information: Primed style effects on recall, interpersonal expectations and affect. Personal Relationships, 10(1), 59–75. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Rule, N. O. , Macrae, C. N., Ambady, N. (2009). Ambiguous group membership is extracted automatically from faces. Psychological Science, 20, 441–443. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Singer, T. , Lamm, C. (2009). The social neuroscience of empathy. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences, 1156, 81–96. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Tottenham, N. , Tanaka, J. , Leon, A. C., McCarry, T., Nurse, M., Marcus, D. J. , ... Nelson, C. (2009). The NimStim set of facial expressions: Judgments from untrained research participants. Psychiatry Research, 168, 242–249. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Van Breukelen, G. J. P., Van Dijk, K. R. A. (2007). Use of covariates in randomized controlled trials. Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society, 13, 903–904. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Vingerhoets, A. J. J. M., Boelhouwer, A. J. W., Van Tilburg, M. A. L. , Van Heck, G. L. (2001). The situational and emotional context of adult crying. In A. J. J. M. Vingerhoets R. CorneliusEds., Adult crying: A biopsychosocial approach (pp. 71–89). Hove, UK: Brunner-Routledge. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Vingerhoets, A. J. J. M., Cornelius, R. R. (2001). Adult crying: A biopsychosocial approach. Philadelphia, PA: Brunner-Routledge. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Wagner, R. E. , Hexel, M. , Bauer, W. W., Kropiunigg, U. (1997). Crying in hospitals: a survey of doctors’, nurses’ and medical students’ experience and attitudes. The Medical Journal of Australia, 166, 13–16. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Whelan, R. (2008). Effective analysis of reaction time data. The Psychological Record, 58, 475–482. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar