Skip to main content
Published Online:https://doi.org/10.1026/1616-3443.38.4.240

Zusammenfassung.Theoretischer Hintergrund: Persistierendes Grübeln über Ursachen und Konsequenzen negativen Erlebens – Rumination – ist ein nachgewiesener Prädiktor für den Beginn und einen ungünstigen Verlauf depressiver Störungen. Erste experimentelle Induktionsstudien geben Hinweise zu hilfreichen antiruminativen Interventionsstrategien. Fragestellung: Welchen Einfluss haben Achtsamkeitsübung und Dezentrierung auf affektive und kognitive Indikatoren induzierter Rumination? Methode: Nach kombinierter Stimmungs- und Ruminationsinduktion wurden 74 nicht-depressive Probanden in drei Interventionsbedingungen randomisiert (Achtsamkeitsübung, Dezentrierung, Kombination). Maße für Stimmung und aktuelle Ruminationstendenz wurden zwischen den Gruppen und longitudinal verglichen. Die Spezifität des Gedächtniszugriffs wird durch ein im deutschen Sprachraum neues Paradigma erfasst. Ergebnisse: Sowohl die erfolgreich induzierte Stimmungsverschlechterung wie die aktuelle Ruminationsneigung verbesserten sich durch alle drei Interventionen bedeutsam. Probanden nach reiner Achtsamkeitsübung hatten den höchsten Anteil spezifischer Erinnerungen. Schlussfolgerungen: Die gezeigte Veränderbarkeit von Ruminationsfolgen im Labor ermutigt weitere Studien zu günstigen Bedingungen für das Beenden persistierenden Gedankenkreisens bei dysphorischen/depressiven Personen.


The influence of mindfulness exercises and decentring on rumination and specificity of autobiographical memory

Abstract.Background: Persistent negative thinking about causes and consequences of negative feelings – rumination – has been shown to predict the onset of depressive disorder and an unfavorable course. Recent experimental studies indicate the potential of antiruminative strategies. Objective: What is the impact of mindfulness exercises and decentring on affective and cognitive consequences of induced rumination? Methods: Following a combined mood and rumination induction 74 nonclinical participants were randomized into three intervention conditions (mindfulness exercises, decentring, combination). Mood and state rumination measures were compared within and between groups. A new paradigm was adapted for the German language to assess specificity of autobiographical memory. Results: After a successful induction procedure all three interventions produced significant improvements in mood and state rumination. Participants in the mindfulness condition generated the highest number of specific memories. Conclusions: The demonstrated changeability of rumination consequences in the laboratory supports future studies aimed at intervention strategies to help dysphoric/depressed people in terminating rumination.

Literatur

  • Abramson, L. Y. , Alloy, L. B. , Hankin, B. L. , Haeffel, G. J. , MacCoon, D. G. , Gibb, B. E. (2002). Cognitive vulnerability-stress models of depression in a self-regulatory and psychobiological context. In I. H. Gotlib, C. H. Hammen (Eds.), Handbook of depression (pp. 268–294). New York: Guilford Press. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Barnard, P. J. , Watkins, E. R. , Ramponi, C. (2006). Reducing specifity of autobiographical memory in nonclinical participants: The role of rumination and schematic models. Cognition and Emotion, 20 (3/4), 328–350. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Beck, A. T. , Epstein, N. , Brown, G. , Steer, R. A. (1988). An inventory for measuring clinical anxiety: Psychometric properties. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 56, 893–897. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Beck, A. T. , Steer, R. A. , Brown, G. K. (1996). Manual for the Beck Depression Inventory, 2nd ed. San Antonio, TX: The Psychological Corporation. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Brewer, D. , Doughtie, E. B. , Lubin, B. (1980). Induction of mood and mood shift. Journal of Clinical Psychology, 36, 215–225. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Broderick, P. (2005). Mindfulness and coping with dysphoric mood: Contrasts with rumination and distraction. Cognitive Therapy and Research, 29, 501–510. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Bürger, C. , Kühner, C. (2007). Copingstile im Umgang mit depressiver Stimmung Faktorenstruktur und psychometrische Gütekriterien der deutschen Version des Response Styles Questionnaires (RSQ). Zeitschrift für Klinische Psychologie und Psychotherapie, 36 (1), 36–45. First citation in articleLinkGoogle Scholar

  • Clark, D. M. , Teasdale, J. D. (1985). Constrains on the effects of mood on memory. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 48, 1595–1608. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Golden, A.-M. , Dalgleish, T. , Mackintosh, B. (2007). Levels of specificity of autobiographical memories and of biographical memories of the deceased in bereaved individuals with and without complicated grief. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 116, 786–795. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Hautzinger, M. , Keller, F. , Kühner, C. (2006). BDI-II: Beck Depressions-Inventar Revision. Frankfurt am Main: Harcourt Test Services. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Kabat-Zinn, J. (1999). Stressbewältigung durch die Praxis der Achtsamkeit (Buch und CD). Deutschland: Arbor Verlag. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Krohne, H. W. , Egloff, B. , Kohlmann, C. W. , Tausch, A. (1996). Untersuchungen mit einer deutschen Version der „Positive and Negative Affect Schedule” (PANAS). Diagnostica, 42 (2), 139–156. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Kühner, C. , Huffziger, S. , Liebsch, K. (2008). Rumination and distraction and mindful self-focus: effects on mood, dysfunctional attitudes and cortisol stress response. Psychological Medicine, 1–10. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Kühner, C. , Huffziger, S. , Nolen-Hoeksema, S. (2007). German Version of the Response Styles Questionnaire (RSQ-D). Manual. Göttingen: Hogrefe. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Lyubomirsky, S. , Nolen-Hoeksema, S. (1995). Effects of self-focused rumination on negative thinking and interpersonal problem solving. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 69, 176–190. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Lyubomirsky, S. , Tkach, C. (2004). The consequences of dysphoric rumination. In C. Papageorgiou, A. Wells (Eds.), Depressive Rumination. Nature, Theory and Treatment (pp. 21–42). Wiley: Chichester. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Margraf, J. , Ehlers, A. (2007). Beck-Angst-Inventar. Deutschsprachige Adaptation des Beck Anxiety Inventory von A. T. Beck und R. A. Stern. Göttingen: Hogrefe. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Martin, L. L. , Tesser, A. (1989). Toward a motivational and structural theory of ruminative thought. In J. S. Uleman, J. A. Bargh (Eds.), Unintended thought (pp. 306–326). New York: Guilford Press. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Martin, M. (1990). On the induction of mood. Clinical Psychology Review, 10, 669–697. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Meibert, P. (2002). Der Einfluss der Achtsamkeitsmeditation auf Linking und Symptombelastung. Unveröff. Dipl. Arbeit, Ruhr-Universität, Bochum. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Morrow, J. , Nolen-Hoeksema, S. (1990). Effects of responses to depression on the remediation of depressive affect. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 58, 519–527. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Nolen-Hoeksema, S. (1991). Responses to depression and their effects on the duration of depressive episodes. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 100, 569–582. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Nolen-Hoeksema, S. (2004). The Response Styles Theory. In C. Papageorgiou, A. Wells (Eds.), Depressive Rumination. Nature, Theory and Treatment (pp. 107– 124). Chichester: Wiley. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Nolen-Hoeksema, S. , Morrow, J. (1993). Effects of rumination and distraction on naturally occurring depressed mood. Cognition & Emotion, 7, 561–570. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Nolen-Hoeksema, S. , Morrow, J. , Fredrickson, B. L. (1993). Response styles and the duration of episodes of depressed mood. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 102, 20–28. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Papageorgiou, C. , Wells, A. (2004). Depressive Rumination. Nature, Theory and Treatment. Chichester: Wiley. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Raes, F. , Hermans, D. , Williams, J. M. G. , Eelen, P. (2007). A sentence completion procedure as an alternative to the Autobiographical Memory Test for assessing overgeneral memory in non-clinical populations. Memory, 15 (5), 495–507. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Singer, A. R. , Dobson, K. S. (2007). An experimental investigation of the cognitive vulnerability to depression. Behavior Research and Therapy, 45, 563–575. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Singer, J. A. , Moffitt, K. H. (1992). An experimental investigation of specificity and generality in memory narratives. Imagination, Cognition, and Personality, 11, 233–257. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Stöber, J. , Borkovec, T. D. (2002). Reduced concreteness of worry in generalized anxiety disorder: Findings from a therapy study. Cognitive Therapy and Research, 26, 89–96. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Teasdale, J. D. (2004). Mindfulness-based Cognitiv Therapy. In J. Yind (Ed.), Cognition, Emotion and Psychopathology: Theoretical, Empirical and Clinical Directions (chap. 13). NY Cambridge: University Press. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Teasdale, J. D. , Moore, R. G. , Hayhurst, H. , Pope, M. , Williams, S. , Segal, Z. V. (2002). Metacognitive awareness and prevention of relapse in depression: Empirical evidence. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 70, 275–287. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Teasdale, J. D. , Segal, Z. V. , Williams, J. M. , Ridgeway, V. A. , Soulsby, J. M. , Lau, M. A. (2000). Prevention of relapse/ recurrence in major depression by mindfulness-based cognitive therapy. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 68, 615–623. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Watkins, E. , Teasdale, J. D. (2004). Adaptive and maladaptive self-focus in depression. Journal of Affective Disorders, 82, 1–8. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Watkins, E. , Teasdale, J. D. , Williams, R. M. (2000). Decentring and distraction reduce overgeneral autobiographical memory in depression. Psychological Medicine, 30, 911–920. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Watson, D. , Clark, L. A. , Tellegen, A. (1988). Development and validation of brief measures of positive and negative affect: The PANAS scales. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 54 (6), 1063–1070. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Wenzlaff, R. M. , Wegner, D. M. (2000). Thought suppression. Annual Review of Psychology, 51, 59–91. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Williams, J. M. G. , Barnhofer, T. , Crane, C. , Hermans, D. , Raes, F. , Watkins, E. , Dalgleish, T. (2007). Autobiographical memory specifity and emotional disorder. Psychological Bulletin, 133, 122–148. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Williams, J. M. G. , Broadbent, K. (1986). Autobiographical memory in attempted suicide patients. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 95, 144–149. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar