Skip to main content
Full-Length Research Report

Different Future Time Perspectives Interplay in Predicting Life Satisfaction

Published Online:https://doi.org/10.1024/1662-9647/a000192

Abstract. This study addresses prior mixed findings on the relationship between future time perspective (FTP) and well-being as well as examines the associations between three aspects of FTP and life satisfaction in the health and friendship domains. 159 Germans, 97 US Americans, and 240 Hong Kong Chinese, aged 19–86 years, completed a survey on future self-views (valence) and life satisfaction. They also reported the extent to which they perceived future time as expanded vs. limited (time extension) and meaningful (openness). Findings revealed that individuals with more positive future self-views had higher satisfaction. However, those who perceived their future as more meaningful or perceived more time in their future reported higher satisfaction even when future self-views were less positive.

References

  • Allemand, M. (2008). Age differences in forgivingness: The role of future time perspective. Journal of Research in Personality, 42, 1137–1147. doi 10.1016/j.jrp.2008.02.009 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Bohn, L., Kwong See, S. T., & Fung, H. H. (2016). Time perspective and positivity effects in Alzheimer’s disease. Psychology and Aging, 31, 574–582. doi 10.1037/pag0000084 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Brandtstädter, J., & Rothermund, K. (2003). Intentionality and time in human development and aging: Compensation and goal adjustment in changing developmental contexts. In U. M. StaudingerU. LindenbergerEds., Understanding human development: Dialogs with life-span psychology (pp. 103–24). Dordrecht, NL: Kluwer. doi 10.1007/978-1-4615-0357-6_6 First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Brandtstädter, J., Rothermund, K., Kranz, D., & Kühn, W. (2010). Final decentrations: Personal goals, rationality perspectives, and the awareness of life’s finitude. European Psychologist, 15, 152–63. doi 10.1027/1016-9040/a000019 First citation in articleLinkGoogle Scholar

  • Brandtstädter, J., & Wentura, D. (1994). Veränderungen der Zeit- und Zukunftsperspektive im Übergang zum höheren Erwachsenenalter: entwicklungspsychologische und differentielle Aspekte [Changes in time and future perspective in the transition to old age: Developmental and differential aspects]. Zeitschrift für Entwicklungspsychologie und Pädagogische Psychologie, 26, 2–21. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Carstensen, L. L. (2006). The influence of a sense of time on human development. Science, 312, 1913–1915. doi 10.1126/science.1127488 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Carstensen, L. L., Isaacowitz, D. M., & Charles, S. T. (1999). Taking time seriously: A theory of socioemotional selectivity. American Psychologist, 54, 165–181. doi 10.1037/0003-066X.54.3.165 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Carstensen, L. L., & Lang, F. R. (1996). Future Time Perspective Scale. Unpublished manuscript, Stanford University. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Cheung, F., & Lucas, R. E. (2015). When does money matter most? Examining the association between income and life satisfaction over the life course. Psychology and Aging, 30, 120–135. doi 10.1037/a0038682 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Coser, L., & Coser, R. (1990). Time perspective and social structure. In J. HassardEd., The sociology of time. London: Macmillan. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Cottle, T. J., & Pleck, J. H. (1969). Linear estimations of temporal extension. The effect of age, sex and social class. Journal of Protective Techniques and Personality Assessment, 33, 81–93. doi 10.1080/0091651X.1969.10120563 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Cross, S. E., & Markus, H. (1991). Possible selves across the life-span. Human Development, 34, 230–255. doi 10.1159/000277058 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Ersner-Hershfield, H., Carvel, D. S., & Isaacowitz, D. M. (2009). Feeling happy and sad, but only seeing the positive: Poignancy and the positivity effect in attention. Motivation and Emotion, 33, 333–342. doi 10.1007/s11031-009-9140-6 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Faul, F., Erdfelder, E., Lang, A. G., & Buchner, A. (2007). G* Power 3: A flexible statistical power analysis program for the social, behavioral, and biomedical sciences. Behavior Research Methods, 39, 175–191. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Fung, H. H., & Carstensen, L. L. (2006). Goals change when life’s fragility is primed: Lessons learned from older adults, the September 11 attacks and SARS. Social Cognition, 24, 248–278. doi 10.1521/soco.2006.24.3.248 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Fung, H. H., Carstensen, L. L., & Lang, F. R. (2001). Age-related patterns in social networks among European Americans and African Americans: Implications for socioemotional selectivity across the life span. International Journal of Aging and Human Development, 52, 185–206. doi 10.2190/1ABL-9BE5-M0X2-LR9V First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Fung, H. H., Carstensen, L. L., & Lutz, A. M. (1999). Influence of time on social preferences: implications for life-span development. Psychology and Aging, 14, 595–604. doi 10.1037/0882-7974.14.4.595 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Fung, H. H., & Isaacowitz, D. M. (2016). The role of time and time perspective in age-related processes: Introduction to the special issue. Psychology and Aging, 31, 553–557. doi 10.1037/pag0000119 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Fung, H. H., Lai, P., & Ng, R. (2001). Age differences in social preferences among Taiwanese and Mainland Chinese: the role of perceived time. Psychology and Aging, 16, 351–356. doi 10.1037/0882-7974.16.2.351 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Grühn, D., Sharifian, N., & Chu, Q. (2016). The limits of a limited future time perspective in explaining age differences in emotional functioning. Psychology and Aging, 31, 583–593. doi 10.1037/pag0000060 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Hess, T. M., O’Brien, E. L., Voss, P., Kornadt, A. E., Rothermund, K., Fung, H. H., & Popham, L. E. (2017). Psychology and aging. Advance online publication. doi 10.1037/pag0000181 First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Hancock, P. A. (2010). The effect of age and sex on the perception of time in life. The American Journal of Psychology, 123(1), 1–13. doi 10.5406/amerjpsyc.123.1.0001 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Hoppmann, C. A., Infurna, F. J., Ram, N., & Gerstorf, D. (2015). Associations among individuals’ perceptions of future time, individual resources, and subjective well-being in old age. The Journals of Gerontology: Series B, Psychological Sciences and Social Sciences. Retrieved from http://psychogerontology.oxfordjournals.org/content/early/2015/10/05/geronb.gbv063.short?rss=1 First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Isaacowitz, D. M., Toner, K., & Neupert, S. D. (2009). Use of gaze for real-time mood regulation: Effects of age and attentional functioning. Psychology and Aging, 24, 989–984. doi 10.1037/a0017706 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Kastenbaum, R. (1961). The dimensions of future time perspective: An experimental analysis. Journal of Genetic Psychology, 65, 203–221. doi 10.1080/00221309.1961.9920473 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Kitayama, S., Markus, H. R., Matsumoto, H., & Norasakkunkit, V. (1997). Individual and collective processes in the construction of the self: self-enhancement in the United States and self-criticism in Japan. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 72, 1245–1267. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Kornadt, A. E., & Rothermund, K. (2011). Contexts of aging: Assessing evaluative age stereotypes in different life domains. Journals of Gerontology Series B: Psychological Sciences and Social Sciences, 66B, 547–556. doi 10.1093/geronb/gbr036 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Kornadt, A. E., & Rothermund, K. (2012). Internalization of age stereotypes into the self-concept via future self-views: A general model and domain-specific differences. Psychology and Aging, 27, 164–172. doi 10.1037/a0025110 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Kornadt, A. E., & Rothermund, K. (2015). Views on aging: Domain-specific approaches and implications for developmental regulation. Annual Review of Gerontology and Geriatrics, 35(1), 121–44. doi 10.1891/0198-8794.35.121 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Kornadt, A. E., Voss, P., & Rothermund, K. (2017a). Age stereotypes and self-views revisited: Patterns of internalization and projection processes across the life span. Journals of Gerontology: Psychological Sciences, 72, 582–92. doi 10.1093/geronb/gbv099 First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Kornadt, A. E., Voss, P., & Rothermund, K. (2017b). Subjective remaining lifetime and concreteness of the future as differential predictors of preparation for age-related changes. European Journal of Ageing. Advance online publication. doi 10.1007/s10433-017-0426-3 First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Lang, A. (2000). The limited capacity model of mediated message processing. Journal of Communication, 50, 46–70. doi 10.1111/j.1460-2466.2000.tb02833.x First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Lang, F. R., & Carstensen, L. L. (1994). Close emotional relationships in late life: Further support for proactive aging in the social domain. Psychology and Aging, 9, 315–324. doi 10.1037/0882-7974.9.2.315 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Lang, F. R., & Carstensen, L. L. (2002). Time counts: Future time perspective, goals, and social relationships. Psychology and Aging, 17, 125–139. doi 10.1037/0882-7974.17.1.125 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Lang, F. R., & Damm, F. (2017). Perceiving future time across adulthood. In G. OettingenA. T. SevincerP. M. GollwitzerEds., The psychology of thinking about the future. New York: Guilford. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Notthoff, N., & Carstensen, L. L. (2014). Positive messaging promotes walking in older adults. Psychology and Aging, 29, 329–341. doi 10.1037/a0036748. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Nuttin, J. (1985). Future time perspective and motivation. Leuven/Hillsdale, NJ: Leuven University Press/Erlbaum. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Nurmi, J. (1991). How do adolescents see their future? A review of the development of future orientation and planning. Developmental Review, 11, 1–59. doi 10.1016/0273-2297(91)90002-6 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • O’Brien, E. L., Hess, T. M., Kornadt, A. E., Rothermund, K., Fung, H., & Voss, P . (2017). Context influences on the subjective experience of aging: The impact of culture and domains of functioning. Gerontologist, 57, S127–S137. doi 10.1093/geront/gnx015 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Poon, C. Y. M, & Fung, H. H. (2008). Physical activity and psychological well-being among Hong Kong Chinese older adults: Exploring the moderating role of self-construal. The International Journal of Aging and Human Development, 66, 1–19. doi 10.2190/AG.66.1.a First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • R Core Team. (2015). R: A language and environment for statistical computing [Computer software]. Vienna, Austria: R Foundation for Statistical Computing. Retrieved from http://www.R-project.org First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Rohr, M. K., John, D. T., Fung, H. H., & Lang, F. R. (2017). A three-component model of future time perspective across adulthood. Psychology and Aging, 32, 597–607. doi 10.1037/pag0000191 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Rothermund, K., & Brandtstädter, J. (1998). Auswirkungen von Belastungen und Verlusten auf die Lebensqualität: alters- und lebenszeitgebundene Moderationseffekte [Impact of negative life changes and losses on subjective life quality: Buffering effects of age and residual life time]. Zeitschrift für Klinische Psychologie, 27, 86–92. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Rothermund, K., & Brandtstädter, J. (2003). Age stereotypes and self-views in later life: Evaluating rival assumptions. International Journal of Behavioral Development, 27, 549–554. doi 10.1080/01650250344000208 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • semTools Contributors. (2016). semTools: Useful tools for structural equation modeling [Computer software]. Retrieved from http://cran.r-project.org/package=semTools First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Voss, P., Wolff, J. K., & Rothermund, K. (2017). Relations between views on aging and perceived age discrimination: A domain-specific perspective. European Journal of Ageing, 14, 5–15. doi 10.1007/s10433-016-0381-4 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar