Skip to main content
Log in

Effects of Time Pressure on Mechanisms of Speech Production and Self-Monitoring

  • Published:
Journal of Psycholinguistic Research Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

The purpose of the present study was to examine effects of time pressure on mechanisms of speech production and self-monitoring. The most widely accepted monitoring theory (Levelt, 1989) suggests that monitoring proceeds through language perception, that is, speech error detection is primarily based on the parsing of one's own inner and overt speech. Twenty-four subjects described visual networks at two different rates (normal and fast). The time pressure manipulation affected a number of temporal characteristics: the error to cutoff and cutoff to repair times were shorter in the fast than in the normal condition. The results indicate that the monitor adjusts its speed of error detection and repair planning to the faster speech output rate. The time pressure manipulation did not affect the accuracy of error detection. The implications for the perception theory of monitoring are discussed.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Similar content being viewed by others

REFERENCES

  • Baddeley, A. D., & Hitch, G. (1974). Working memory. In G. A. Bower (Ed.), The psychology of learning and motivation (pp. 47-89). New York: Academic Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Berg, T. (1986). The aftermath of error occurrence: psycholinguistic evidence from cut offs. Language and Communication, 6, 195-213.

    Google Scholar 

  • Blackmer, E. R., & Mitton, J. L. (1991). Theories of monitoring and the timing of repairs in spontaneous speech. Cognition, 39, 173-194.

    Google Scholar 

  • Brédart, S. (1991). Word-interruption in self-repairing. Journal of Psycholinguistic Research, 20, 123-138.

    Google Scholar 

  • Christenfeld, N. (1994). Options and ums. Journal of Language and Social Psychology, 13, 192-199.

    Google Scholar 

  • Clark, H. H., & Clark, E. (1977). Psychology and Language.An Introduction to Psycholinguistics. Harcourt, New York.

    Google Scholar 

  • Clark, H. H., & Wasow, W. (1998). Repeating words in spontaneous speech. Cognitive Psychology, 37, 201-242.

    Google Scholar 

  • Dell, G. S. (1986). A spreading activation theory of retrieval in sentence production. Psychological Review, 93, 124-142.

    Google Scholar 

  • Dell, G. S. (1990). Effects of frequency and vocabulary type on phonological speech errors. Language and Cognitive Processes, 5, 313-349.

    Google Scholar 

  • Dell, G. S., & Repka, R. J. (1992). Errors in inner speech. In B. J. Baars (Ed.), Experimental slips and human error: Exploring the architecture of volition. New York: Plenum Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • De Smedt, K., & Kempen, G. (1987). Incremental sentence production, self-correction and coordination. In G. Kempen (Ed.), Natural language generation (pp. 365-376). Dordrecht: Martinus Nijhoff.

    Google Scholar 

  • Feldstein, S, Crown, C. L., & Jaffe, J. (1991). Expectation and extraversion: influencing the perceived rate of tone-silence sequences. Bulletin of the Psychonomic Society, 29, 395-398.

    Google Scholar 

  • Garnsey, S. M., & Dell, G. S. (1984). Some neurolinguistic implications of prearticulatory editing in production. Brain and Language, 23, 64-73.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gathercole, S. E., & Baddeley, A. D. (1993). Working memory and language. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Goldman-Eisler, F. (1968). Psycholinguistics: Experiments in spontaneous speech. New York: Academic Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Grosjean, F., & Lane, H. (1981). Temporal variables in the perception and production of spoken and sign languages. In P. D. Eimas & J. L. Miller(Eds.), Perspectives in the study of speech (pp. 207-238). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hartsuiker, R. J., & Kolk, H. H. J. (in press). Error monitoring in speech production: a computational test of the perceptual loop theory. Cognitive Psychology.

  • Henderson, A. I. (1974). Time patterns in spontaneous speech-cognitive stride or random walk? A reply to Jaffe et al. (1972). Language and Speech, 17, 119-125.

    Google Scholar 

  • Howell, P., Kadi-Hanifi, K., & Joung, K. (1991). Phrase revisions in fluent and stuttering children. In H. M. Peters, W. Hulstijn, & C. W. Starkweather (Eds.), Speech motor control and stuttering. Amsterdam: Elsevier.

    Google Scholar 

  • Jaffe, J., Anderson, S. W., & Rieber, R. W. (1973). Research and clinical approaches to disorders of speech rate. Journal of Communication Disorders, 6, 225-246.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kempen, G. (submitted). Human Grammatical Coding. Book manuscript.

  • Kempen, G., & Hoenkamp, E. (1987). An incremental procedure grammar for sentence formulation. Cognitive Science, 11, 201-258.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kemper, S. (1992). Adults' sentence fragments: who, what, when, where, and why? Communication Research, 19, 444-458.

    Google Scholar 

  • Klapp, S. T., Anderson, W. G., & Berrian, R. W. (1973). Implicit speech in reading, reconsidered. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 100, 368-374.

    Google Scholar 

  • Klapp, S. T., & Erwin, C. I. (1976). Relation between programming time and duration of the response being programmed. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 2, 59-598.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kowall, S., O'Connell, D. C., & Sabin, E. J. (1975). Temporal patterning and vocal hesitations in spontaneous narratives. Journal of Psycholinguistic Research, 4, 195-204.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lackner, J. R., & Tuller, B. H. (1979). Role of efference monitoring in the detection of selfproduced speech errors. In W. E. Cooper & E. C. T. Walker(Eds.), Sentence processing (pp. 281-294). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Laver, J. D. M. (1980). Monitoring systems in the neurolinguistica control of speech production. In V. A. Fromkin (Ed.), Errors in linguistic performance: Slips of the tongue, ear, pen, and hand (pp. 287-305). New York: Academic Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Levelt, W. J. M. (1983). Monitoring in self-repair in speech. Cognition, 14, 41-104.

    Google Scholar 

  • Levelt, W. J. M. (1989). Speaking: From intention to articulation. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Levelt, W. J. M. (1992). The perceptual loop theory not disconfirmed: A reply to MacKay. Consciousness and Cognition, 1, 226-230.

    Google Scholar 

  • Levelt, W. J. M., Roelofs, A., & Meyer, A. S. (1999). A theory of lexical access in speech production. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 22, 1-37.

    Google Scholar 

  • MacKay, D. G. (1982). The problems of flexibility, fluency, and speed-accuracy trade-off in skilled behavior. Psychological Review, 89, 483-506.

    Google Scholar 

  • MacKay, D. G. (1987). The organization of perception and action: A theory for language and other cognitive skills. New York: Springer.

    Google Scholar 

  • MacKay, D. G. (1992a). Errors, ambiguity, and awareness in language perception and production. In B. J. Baars(Ed.), Experimental slips and human error: Exploring the architecture of volition. New York: Plenum Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • MacKay, D. G. (1992b). Awareness and error detection: New theories and research paradigms. Consciousness and Cognition, 1, 199-225.

    Google Scholar 

  • Marslen-Wilson, W., & Tyler, L. (1981). Central processes in speech understanding. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society London, B259, 317-332.

    Google Scholar 

  • Martin, N., Weisberg, R. W., & Saffran, E. M. (1989). Variables influencing the occurrence of naming errors: implications for models of lexical retrieval. Journal of Memory and Language, 28, 462-485.

    Google Scholar 

  • Miller, J. L., Grosjean, F., & Lomanto, C. (1984). Articulation rate and its variability in spontaneous speech: a reanalysis and some implications. Phonetica, 41, 215-225.

    Google Scholar 

  • Miyake, A., Carpenter, P. A., & Just, M. A. (1995). Reduced resources and specific inpairments in normal and aphasic sentence comprehension. Cognitive Neuropsychology, 12, 651-679.

    Google Scholar 

  • Motley, M. T. (1980). Verification of "Freudian slips" and semantic prearticulatory editing via laboratory-induced spoonerisms. In V. A. Fromkin (Ed.), Errors in linguistic performance: Slips of the tongue, ear, pen, and hand. New York: Academic Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Motley, M. T., Camden, C. T., & Baars, B. J. (1982). Covert formulation and editing of anomalies in speech production: Evidence from experimentally elicited slips of the tongue.Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 21, 578-594

    Google Scholar 

  • Norman, D. A., & Shallice, T. (1986). Attention to action: Willed and automatic control of behavior. In R. J. Davidson, G. E. Schwarts, & D. Shapiro (Eds.), Consciousness and Self-Regulation, Vol. 4 (pp. 1-18). London: Wiley.

    Google Scholar 

  • Postma, A. (2000). Detection of Errors during Speech Production: A Review of Speech Monitoring Models. Cognition, 77, 97-131.

    Google Scholar 

  • Postma, A., & Kolk, H. H. J. (1992). The effects of noise masking and required accuracy on speech errors, disfluencies, and self-repairs. Journal of Speech and Hearing Research, 35, 537-544.

    Google Scholar 

  • Postma, A., & Kolk, H. H. J. (1993). The covert repair hypothesis: prearticulatory repair processes in normal and stuttered disfluencies. Journal of Speech and Hearing Research, 36, 472-487.

    Google Scholar 

  • Postma, A., Kolk, H., & Povel, D. J. (1990). On the relation among speech errors, disfluencies, and self-repairs. Language and Speech, 33, 19-29.

    Google Scholar 

  • Postma, A., & Noordanus, C. (1996). The production and detection of speech errors in silent, mouthed, noise-masked, and normal auditory feedback speech. Language and Speech, 39, 375-392.

    Google Scholar 

  • Schlenck, K. J., Huber, W., & Willmes, K. (1987). Prepairs and repairs: different monitoring functions in aphasic language production. Brain and Language, 30, 226-244.

    Google Scholar 

  • Shriberg, E. E. (1994). Preliminaries to a theory of speech disfluencies. Unpublished doctoral dissertation.

  • Van Hest, G. W. C. M. (1996). Self-repair in L1 and L2 production. In R. Appel, G. Extra, K. Jaspaert, & L. Verhoeven (Eds.), Studies in multilingualism, Vol. 4, Tilburg: University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Van Wijk, C., & Kempen, G. (1987). A dual system for producing self-repairs in spontaneous speech: evidence from experimentally elicited corrections. Cognitive Psychology, 19, 403-440.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wheeldon, L. R., & Levelt, W. J. M. (1995). Monitoring the time course of phonological encoding. Journal of Memory and Language, 34, 311-334.

    Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Cite this article

Oomen, C.C.E., Postma, A. Effects of Time Pressure on Mechanisms of Speech Production and Self-Monitoring. J Psycholinguist Res 30, 163–184 (2001). https://doi.org/10.1023/A:1010377828778

Download citation

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1023/A:1010377828778

Navigation