Skip to main content
Log in

Risky Business: Emotion, Decision-Making, and Addiction

  • Published:
Journal of Gambling Studies Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

Although metabolic abnormalities in the orbitofrontal cortex have been observed in substance dependent individuals (SDI) for several years, very little attention was paid to the role of this brain region in addiction. However, patients with damage to the ventromedial (VM) sector of the prefrontal cortex and SDI show similar behaviors. (1) They often deny, or they are not aware, that they have a problem. (2) When faced with a choice to pursue a course of action that brings an immediate reward at the risk of incurring future negative consequences, they choose the immediate reward and ignore the future consequences. Studies of patients with bilateral lesions of the VM prefrontal cortex support the view that the process of decision-making depends in many important ways on neural substrates that regulate homeostasis, emotion, and feeling. Parallel lines of study have revealed that VM cortex dysfunction is also evident in subgroups of individuals who are addicted to substances. Thus, understanding the neural mechanisms of decision-making has direct implications for understanding disorders of addiction and pathological gambling, and the switch from a controlled to uncontrolled and compulsive behavior. On the clinical front, the approach to treat addictive disorders has been dominated by a diagnostic system that focuses on behaviors, physical symptoms, or choice of drugs. The article emphasizes the concept of using neurocognitive criteria for subtyping addictive disorders. This is a significant paradigm shift with significant implications for guiding diagnosis and treatment. Using neurocognitive criteria could lead to more accurate subtyping of addictive disorders, and perhaps serve as a guide for more specific, and potentially more successful, behavioral and pharmacological interventions.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Similar content being viewed by others

References

  • Ackerly, S. S., & Benton, A. L. (1948). Report of a case of bilateral frontal lobe defect. Proceedings of the Association for Research in Nervous and Mental Disease (Baltimore), 27, 479–504.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ainslie, G. (2000). A research-based theory of addictive motivation. Law and Philosophy, 19(1), 77–115.

    Google Scholar 

  • Anderson, S. W., Damasio, H., Jones, R. D., & Tranel, D. (1991). Wisconsin card sorting test performance as a measure of frontal lobe damage. Journal of Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology, 3, 909–922.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bartzokis, G., Lu, P. H., Beckson, M., Rapoport, R., Grant, S., Wiseman, E. J., & London, E. D. (2000). Abstinence from cocaine reduces high-risk responses on a gambling task. Neuropsychopharmacology, 22(1), 102–103.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bechara, A., Damasio, A. R., Damasio, H., & Anderson, S. W. (1994). Insensitivity to future consequences following damage to human prefrontal cortex. Cognition, 50, 7–15.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bechara, A., & Damasio, H. (2002). Decision-Making And Addiction (Part I): Impaired Activation of Somatic States in Substance Dependent Individuals when Pondering Decisions with Negative Future Consequences. Neuropsychologia, 40(10), 1675–1689.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bechara, A., Damasio, H., & Damasio, A. (2001). Manipulation of dopamine and serotonin causes different effects on covert and overt decision-making. Society for Neuroscience Abstracts, 27.

  • Bechara, A., Damasio, H., & Damasio, A. R. (2000). Emotion, decision-making, and the orbitofrontal cortex. Cerebral Cortex, 10(3), 295–307.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bechara, A., Damasio, H., Damasio, A. R., & Lee, G. P. (1999). Different contributions of the human amygdala and ventromedial prefrontal cortex to decision-making. The Journal of Neuroscience, 19(13), 5473–5481.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bechara, A., Damasio, H., Tranel, D., & Anderson, S. W. (1998). Dissociation of working memory from decision making within the human prefrontal cortex. The Journal of Neuroscience, 18, 428–437.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bechara, A., Dolan, S., Denburg, N., Hindes, A., Anderson, S. W., & Nathan, P. E. (2001). Decisionmaking deficits, linked to a dysfunctional ventromedial prefrontal cortex, revealed in alcohol and stimulant abusers. Neuropsychologia, 39, 376–389.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bechara, A., Dolan, S., & Hindes, A. (2002). Decision-Making and Addiction (Part II): Myopia For The Future Or Hypersensitivity To Reward? Neuropsychologia, 40(10), 1690–1705.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bechara, A., Tranel, D., & Damasio, H. (2000). Characterization of the decision-making impairment of patients with bilateral lesions of the ventromedial prefrontal cortex. Brain, 123, 2189–2202.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bechara, A., Tranel, D., Damasio, H., & Damasio, A. R. (1996). Failure to respond autonomically to anticipated future outcomes following damage to prefrontal cortex. Cerebral Cortex, 6, 215–225.

    Google Scholar 

  • Breiter, H. C., Aharon, I., Kahneman, D., Dale, A., & Shizgal, P. (2001). Functional imaging of neural responses to expectancy and experience of monetary gains and losses. Neuron, 30(2), 619–639.

    Google Scholar 

  • Breiter, H. C., & Rosen, B. R. (1999). Functional magnetic resonance imaging of brain reward circuitry in the human, Advancing From the Ventral Striatum to the Extended Amygdala (Vol. 877, pp. 523–547).

    Google Scholar 

  • Brickner, R. M. (1932). An interpretation of frontal lobe function based upon the study of a case of partial bilateral frontal lobectomy. Localization of function in the cerebral cortex. Proceedings of the Association for Research in Nervous and Mental Disease (Baltimore), 13, 259–351.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bush, G., Luu, P., & Posner, M. I. (2000). Cognitive and emotional influences in anterior cingulate cortex. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 4(6), 215–222.

    Google Scholar 

  • Carter, C. S., Braver, T. S., Barch, D. M., Botvinick, M. M., Noll, D., & Cohen, J. D. (1998). Anterior cingulate cortex, error detection, and the online monitoring of performance. Science, 280 (5364), 747–749.

    Google Scholar 

  • Cavedini, P., D'Annucci, A., Ubbilali, A., Zorzi, C., Bassi, T., Glordani, S., & Bellodi, L. (2001). Pathological gambling and obsessive-compulsive spectrum disorder: neuropsychological evidence. The World Journal of Bilogical Psychiatry, 2(Supplement 1), 2315.

    Google Scholar 

  • Childress, A. R., Mozley, P. D., McElgin, W., Fitzgerald, J., Reivich, M., & O'Brien, C. P. (1999). Limbic activation during cue-induced cocaine craving. American Journal of Psychiatry, 156, 11–18.

    Google Scholar 

  • Cremer von Schinkel, J. (2001). Do pathological gamblers suffer from “brain block”? 7th World Congress of Biological Psychiatry (July 3).

  • Damasio, A. R. (1994). Descartes' Error: Emotion, Reason, and the Human Brain. New York: Grosset/Putnam.

    Google Scholar 

  • Damasio, A. R. (1995). Toward a neurobiology of emotion and feeling: operational concepts and hypotheses. The Neuroscientist, 1, 19–25.

    Google Scholar 

  • Damasio, A. R. (1996). The somatic marker hypothesis and the possible functions of the prefrontal cortex. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society of London (Biology), 351, 1413–1420.

    Google Scholar 

  • Damasio, A. R., Grabowski, T. G., Bechara, A., Damasio, H., Ponto, L. L. B., Parvizi, J., & Hichwa, R. D. (2000). Subcortical and cortical brain activity during the feeling of self-generated emotions. Nature Neuroscience, 3(10), 1049–1056.

    Google Scholar 

  • Damasio, A. R., Tranel, D., & Damasio, H. (1990). Individuals with sociopathic behavior caused by frontal damage fail to respond autonomically to social stimuli. Behavioral Brain Research, 41, 81–94.

    Google Scholar 

  • Damasio, A. R., Tranel, D., & Damasio, H. (1991). Somatic markers and the guidance of behavior: Theory and preliminary testing. In H. S. Levin & H. M. Eisenberg & A. L. Benton (Eds.), Frontal Lobe Function and Dysfunction (pp. 217–229). New York: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Damasio, H. (1995). Human Brain Anatomy in Computerized Images. New York: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Damasio, H., Grabowski, T., Frank, R., Galburda, A. M., & Damasio, A. R. (1994). The return of Phineas Gage: Clues about the brain from the skull of a famous patient. Science, 264, 1102–1104.

    Google Scholar 

  • Devinsky, O., Morrell, M. J., & Vogt, B. A. (1995). Contributions of anterior cingulate cortex to behavior. Brain, 118, 279–306.

    Google Scholar 

  • Dias, R., Robbins, T. W., & Roberts, A. C. (1996). Dissociation in prefrontal cortex of affective and attentional shifts. Nature, 380, 69–72.

    Google Scholar 

  • Dias, R., Robbins, T. W., & Roberts, A. C. (1997). Dissociable forms of inhibitory control within prefrontal cortex with an analog of the Wisconsin Card Sort Test: restrictions to novel situations and independence from “on-line” processing. Journal of Neuroscience, 17, 9285–9297.

    Google Scholar 

  • Eslinger, P. J., & Damasio, A. R. (1985). Severe disturbance of higher cognition after bilateral frontal lobe ablation: patient EVR. Neurology, 35, 1731–1741.

    Google Scholar 

  • Everitt, B. J., Parkinson, J. A., Olmstead, M. C., Arroyo, M., Robledo, P., & Robbins, T. W. (1999). Associative processes in addiction and reward: the role of amygdala and ventral striatal subsystems. Annals of the New York Academy of Science (Advancing from the Ventral Striatum to the Extended Amygdala), 877, 412–438.

    Google Scholar 

  • Frith, C. D., Friston, K., Liddle, P. F., & Frackowiak, R. S. J. (1991). Willed Action and the Prefrontal Cortex in Man—A Study With PET. Proceedings of the Royal Society of London Series B-Biological Sciences, 244 (1311), 241–2

    Google Scholar 

  • Garavan, H., Ross, T. J., & Stein, E. A. (1999). Right hemispheric dominance of inhibitory control: an event-related functional MRI study. Proceedings of the National Academy of Science USA, 96(14), 8301–8306.

  • Grant, S., Contoreggi, C., & London, E. D. (1997). Drug abusers show impaired performance on a test of orbitofrontal function. Society for Neuroscience Abstracts, 23, 1943.

    Google Scholar 

  • Grant, S., Contoreggi, C., & London, E. D. (2000). Drug abusers show impaired performance in a laboratory test of decision-making. Neuropsychologia, 38(8), 1180–1187.

    Google Scholar 

  • Grant, S. J., Bonson, K. R., Contoreggi, C. C., & London, E. D. (1999). Activation of the ventromedial prefrontal cortex correlates with gambling task performance: A FDG-PET study. Society for Neuroscience Abstracts, 25(2), 1551.

    Google Scholar 

  • Green, L., Fristoe, N., & Myerson, J. (1994). Temporal Discounting and Preference Reversals in Choice Between Delayed Outcomes. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 1(3), 383–389.

    Google Scholar 

  • Green, L., Fry, A., & Myerson, J. (1994). Discounting of delayed rewards: a life-span comparison. Psychological Science, 5, 33–36.

    Google Scholar 

  • Harlow, J. M. (1848). Passage of an iron bar through the head. Boston Medical and Surgical Journal, 39, 389–393.

    Google Scholar 

  • Harlow, J. M. (1868). Recovery from the passage of an iron bar through the head. Publications of the Massachusetts Medical Society, 2, 327–347.

    Google Scholar 

  • Heyman, G. M. (1996). Resolving the contradictions of addiction. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 19(4), 561–610.

    Google Scholar 

  • Jentsch, J. D., & Taylor, J. R. (1999). Impulsivity resulting from frontostraital dysfunction in drug abuse: implications for the control of behavior by reward-related stimuli. Psychopharmacology, 146(4), 373–390.

    Google Scholar 

  • Johnson, B. A., Roache, J. D., Javors, M. A., DiClemente, C. C., Cloninger, C. R., Prihoda, T. J., Bordnick, P. S., Ait-Daoud, N., & Hensler, J. (2000). Ondansetron for reduction of drinking among biologically predisposed alcoholic patients—A randomized controlled trial. Jama-Journal of the American Medical Association, 284(8), 963–971.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kirby, K. N., & Herrnstein, R. J. (1995). Preference Reversals Due to Myopic Discounting of Delayed Reward. Psychological Science, 6(2), 83–89.

    Google Scholar 

  • Konishi, S., Kawazu, M., Uchida, I., Kikyo, H., Asakura, I., & Miyashita, Y. (1999). Contribution of working memory to transient activation in human inferior prefrontal cortex during performance of the Wisconsin Card Sorting Test. Cerebral Cortex, 9(7), 745–753.

    Google Scholar 

  • Koob, G. F. (1999). The role of the striatopallidal and extended amygdala systems in drug addiction. Annals of the New York Academy of Science (Advancing from the Ventral Striatum to the Extended Amygdala), 877, 445–460.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lombardi, W. J., Andreason, P. J., Sirocco, K. Y., Rio, D. E., Gross, R. E., Umhau, J. C., & Hommer, D. W. (1999). Wisconsin card sorting test performance following head injury: Dorsolateral fronto-striatal circuit activity predicts perseveration. Journal of Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology, 21(1), 2–16.

    Google Scholar 

  • London, E. D., Ernst, M., Grant, S., Bonson, K., & Weinstein, A. (2000). Orbitofrontal cortex and human drug abuse: functional imaging. Cerebral Cortex, 10(3), 334–342.

    Google Scholar 

  • Maddock, R. J. (1999). The retrosplenial cortex and emotion: new insights from functional neuroimaging of the human brain. Trends in Neurosciences, 22(7), 310–320.

    Google Scholar 

  • Mazas, C. A., Finn, P. R., & Steinmetz, J. E. (2000). Decision making biases, antisocial personality, and early-onset alcoholism. Alcoholism: Clinical and Experimental Research, 24(7), 1036–1040.

    Google Scholar 

  • Milner, B. (1963). Effects of different brain lesions on card sorting. Archives of Neurology, 9, 90–100.

    Google Scholar 

  • Monterosso, J., Ehrman, R., Napier, K., O'Brien, C. P., & Childress, A. R. (2001). Three Decision-Making Tasks in Cocaine-Dependent Patients: Do they measure the same construct? Addiction, 96(12), 1825–1837.

    Google Scholar 

  • Owen, A. M., Roberts, A. C., Hodges, J. R., Summers, B. A., Polkey, C. E., & Robbins, T. W. (1993). Contrasting Mechanisms of Impaired Attentional Set-Shifting in Patients With Frontal-Lobe Damage or Parkinsons-Disease. Brain, 116, 1159–1175.

    Google Scholar 

  • Owen, A. M., Roberts, A. C., Polkey, C. E., Sahakian, B. J., & Robbins, T. W. (1991). Extra-Dimensional Versus Intra-Dimensional Set Shifting Performance Following Frontal-Lobe Excisions, Temporal-Lobe Excisions or Amygdalo-Hippocampectomy in Man. Neuropsychologia, 29(10), 993–1006.

    Google Scholar 

  • Pardo, J. V., Pardo, P. J., Janer, K. W., & Raichle, M. E. (1990). The Anterior Cingulate Cortex Mediates Processing Selection in the Stroop Attentional Conflict Paradigm. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 87(1), 256–259.

  • Petry, N. M. (2001). Substance abuse, pathological gambling, and impulsiveness. Drug and Alcohol Dependence, 63(1), 29–38.

    Google Scholar 

  • Petry, N. M., Bickel, W. K., & Arnett, M. (1998). Shortened time horizons and insensitivity to future consequences in heroin addicts. Addiction, 93(5), 729–738.

    Google Scholar 

  • Potenza, M. N., & Wilber, M. K. (2001). Neuroimaging studies of pathological gambling and substance dependence. Psychiatric Times, 17(10).

  • Rahman, S., Sahakian, B. J., Rudolph, N. C., Rogers, R. D., & Robbins, T. W. (2001). Decision making and neuropsychiatry. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 6(5), 271–277.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rogers, R. D., Everitt, B. J., Baldacchino, A., Blackshaw, A. J., Swainson, R., Wynne, K., Baker, N. B., Hunter, J., Carthy, T., Booker, E., London, M., Deakin, J. F. W., Sahakian, B. J., & Robbins, T. W. (1999). Dissociable deficits in the decision-making cognition of chronic amphetamine abusers, opiate abusers, patients with focal damage to prefrontal cortex, and tryptophandepleted normal volunteers: evidence for monoaminergic mechanisms. Neuropsychopharmacology, 20(4), 322–339.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rogers, R. D., Owen, A. M., Middleton, H. C., Williams, E. J., Pickard, J. D., Sahakian, B. J., & Robbins, T. W. (1999). Choosing between small, likely rewards and large, unlikely rewards activates inferior and orbital prefrontal cortex. Journal of Neuroscience, 20, 9029–9038.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rolls, E. T., Hornak, J., Wade, D., & McGrath, J. (1994). Emotion-related learning in patients with social and emotional changes associated with frontal lobe damage. Journal of Neurology, Neurosurgery, and Psychiatry, 57, 1518–1524.

    Google Scholar 

  • Stapleton, J. M., Morgan, M. J., Phillips, R. L., Wong, D. F., Yung, B. C. K., Shaya, E. K., Dannals, R. F., Liu, X., Grayson, R. L., & London, E. D. (1995). Cerebral glucose utilization in polysubstance abuse. Neuropsychopharmacology, 13(1), 22–31.

    Google Scholar 

  • Volkow, N. D., & Fowler, J. S. (2000). Addiction, a disease of compulsion and drive: involvement of the orbitofrontal cortex. Cerebral Cortex, 10(3), 318–325.

    Google Scholar 

  • Volkow, N. D., Fowler, J. S., Wolf, A. P., Hitzemann, R., Dewey, S., Bendriem, B., Alpert, R., & Hoff, A. (1991). Changes in brain glucose metabolism in cocaine dependence and withdrawal. American Journal of Psychiatry, 148, 621–626.

    Google Scholar 

  • Vuchinich, R. E., & Tucker, J. A. (1988). Contributions From Behavioral Theories of Choice to an Analysis of Alcohol-Abuse. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 97(2), 181–195.

    Google Scholar 

  • Welt, L. (1888). Uber Charaktervaranderungen des Menschen infoldge von Lasionen des Stirnhirns. Dtsch. Arch. Klin. Med., 42, 339–390.

    Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Cite this article

Bechara, A. Risky Business: Emotion, Decision-Making, and Addiction. J Gambl Stud 19, 23–51 (2003). https://doi.org/10.1023/A:1021223113233

Download citation

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1023/A:1021223113233

Navigation