Skip to main content
Log in

Competition for Resources and Its Behavioral Consequences Among Female Primates

  • Published:
International Journal of Primatology Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

Via the current model on the evolutionary ecology of female social relationships, Sterck et al. (1997) argue that ecological conditions determine how competition over food resources affects female fitness. The relative importance of different modes of competition then affects female social relationships and dispersal patterns. I outline the model and review relevant data. There are 3 modes of feeding competition: within-group scramble (WGS), within-group contest (WGC), and between-group contest (BGC), which occur in various combinations in different populations of nonhuman primates. Ecological measures support predictions that limiting resources lead to WGS and clumped resources induce WGC. The ecological basis of BGC remains elusive, but it is probably linked to resource abundance. Tests of the proxies of feeding competition support the idea that short-term search substrates and increasing group size lead to WGS, while high-quality patches of intermediate size relative to group size lead to WGC. However, when tested across populations, independent measures of aggression rates do not always match the actual or presumed competitive regimes. This mismatch might be explained by confounding factors and the predominately indirect measures of feeding competition. Predicted relationships between feeding competition and female social relationships/dispersal are only partly supported. This might be attributed to the fact that few studies have taken ultimate approaches using mechanistic correlates of fitness (net energy gain) or lifetime reproductive success to measure consequences of feeding competition. But to resolve existing inconsistencies, additional factors need to be taken into account as well, for example, male sexual strategies may affect female feeding competition; constraints on group size may enforce female dispersal; and demography may alter rates of alliances. More explicitly, ultimate approaches are needed to test the consistency of the socioecological model.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Similar content being viewed by others

REFERENCES

  • Alexander, R. D. (1974). The evolution of social behavior. Ann. Rev. Ecol. Syst. 5: 325–383.

    Google Scholar 

  • Altman, J., Hausfater, G., and Altmann, S. (1988). Determinants of reproductive success in savannah baboons, Papio cynocephalus. In Clutton-Brock, T. H. (ed.), Reproductive Success, Chicago University Press, Chicago, pp. 403–418.

    Google Scholar 

  • Aureli, F., Das, M., and Veenema, H. C. (1997). Differential kinship effect on reconciliation in three species of macaques (Macaca fascicularis, M. fuscata, and M. sylvanus). J. Comp.Psychol. 111: 91–99.

    Google Scholar 

  • Barnard, C. J., and Silby, R. M. (1981). Producers and scroungers: A general model and its application to captive flocks of house sparrows. Anim. Behav. 29: 543–550.

    Google Scholar 

  • Barta, Z., and Giraldeau, L.-A. (1998). The effect of dominance hierarchy on the use of alternative foraging tactics:Aphenotype-limited producing-scrounging game. Behav. Ecol.Sociobiol. 42: 217–223.

    Google Scholar 

  • Barton, R. A., Byrne, R. W., and Whiten, A. (1996). Ecology, feeding competition and social structure in baboons. Behav. Ecol. Sociobiol. 38: 321–329.

    Google Scholar 

  • Barton, R. A., and Whiten, A. (1993). Feeding competition among female olive baboons, Papio anubis. Anim. Behav. 46: 777–789.

    Google Scholar 

  • Beauchamp, G. (1998). The effect of group size on mean food intake rate in birds. Biol. Rev. 73: 449–472.

    Google Scholar 

  • Boinski, S. (1999). The social organization of squirrel monkeys: Implications for ecological models of social evolution. Evol. Anthropol. 8: 101–112.

    Google Scholar 

  • Borries, C. (1993). Ecology of female social relationships: Hanuman langurs and the van Schaik model. Folia Primatol. 61: 21–30.

    Google Scholar 

  • Borries, C., Sommer, V., and Srivastava, A. (1991). Dominance, age, and reproductive success in free-ranging Hanuman langurs (Presbytis entellus). Int. J. Primatol. 12: 231–257.

    Google Scholar 

  • Chapais, B. (1992). The role of alliances in social inheritance of rank among female primates.In Harcourt, A. H., and DeWaal, F. B. M. (eds.), Coalitions and Alliances in Humans and Other Animals, Oxford University Press, Oxford, pp. 29–59.

    Google Scholar 

  • Chapman, C. A. (1988). Patch use and patch depletion by the spider and howling monkeys of Santa Rosa National Park, Costa Rica. Behaviour 105: 99–116.

    Google Scholar 

  • Chapman, C. A., and Chapman, L. J. (2000). Determinants of group size in primates: The importance of travel costs. In Boinski, S., and Garber, P. A. (eds.), On the Move. How and Why Animals Travel in Groups, University of Chicago Press, Chicago, pp. 24–42.

    Google Scholar 

  • Chapman, C. A., Wrangham, R.W., and Chapman, L. J. (1995). Ecological constraints on group size: An analysis of spider monkey and chimpanzee subgroups. Behav. Ecol. Sociobiol. 36: 59–70.

    Google Scholar 

  • Cheney, D. L., and Seyfarth, R. M. (1987). The influence of intergroup competition on the survival and reproduction of female vervet monkeys. Behav. Ecol. Sociobiol. 21: 375–386.

    Google Scholar 

  • Cheney, D. L., Seyfarth, R. M., Andelman, S. J., and Lee, P. C. (1988). Reproductive success in vervet monkeys. In Clutton-Brock, T. H. (ed.), Reproductive Success, Chicago University Press, Chicago, pp. 384–402.

    Google Scholar 

  • Clark, C. W., and Mangel, M. (1986). The evolutionary advantages of group foraging. Theor.Popul. Biol. 30: 45–75.

    Google Scholar 

  • Clutton-Brock, T. H., and Harvey, P.H. (1977). Primate ecology and social organization. J. Zool.Lond. 183: 1–39.

    Google Scholar 

  • Cords, M. (2000). Agonistic and affiliative relationships in a blue monkey group. InWhitehead, P. F., and Jolly, C. J. (eds.), Old World Monkeys, Cambridge University Press, Cambridge, pp. 453–479.

    Google Scholar 

  • Crockett, C. M., and Janson, C. H. (2000). Infanticide in red howlers: Female group size, male composition, and a possible link to folivory. In van Schaik, C. P., and Janson, C. H. (eds.), Infanticide by Males and its Implications, Cambridge University Press, Cambridge, pp. 75–98.

    Google Scholar 

  • Darwin, C. (1859). The Origin of Species by Means of Natural Selection, John Murray, London.

    Google Scholar 

  • Datta, S. B., and Beauchamp, G. (1991). Effects of group demography on dominance relationships among female primates. I. Mother- daughter and sister- sister relations. Am. Nat. 138: 201–226.

    Google Scholar 

  • de Vries, H. (1995).Animproved test of linearity in dominance hierarchies containing unknown or tied relationships. Anim. Behav. 50: 1375–1389.

    Google Scholar 

  • de Waal, F. B. M., and Luttrell, L. M. (1989). Toward a comparative socioecology of the genus Macaca: Different dominance styles in rhesus and stumptail monkeys. Am. J. Primatol. 19: 83–109.

    Google Scholar 

  • DiFiore, A., and Rendall, D. (1994). Evolution of social organization:Areappraisal for primates by using phylogenetic methods. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA 91: 9941–9945.

    Google Scholar 

  • Digby, L. (2000). Infanticide by female mammals: Implications for the evolution of social systems.In van Schaik, C. P., and Janson, C. H. (eds.), Infanticide by Males and its Implications, Cambridge University Press, Cambridge, pp. 423–446.

    Google Scholar 

  • Dunbar, R. I. M. (1988). Primate Social Systems, Croom Helm, London.

    Google Scholar 

  • Fashing, P. J. (2001). Male and female strategies during intergroup encounters in guerezas (Colobus guereza): Evidence for resource defense mediated through males and a comparison with other primates. Behav. Ecol. Sociobiol. 50: 219–230.

    Google Scholar 

  • Furichi, T. (1983). Interindividual distances and influence of dominance on feeding in natural Japanese macaque troop. Primates 24: 445–455.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gillespie, T. A., and Chapman, C. A. (2001). Determinants of group size in the red colobus monkey (Procolobus badius): An evaluation of the generality of the ecological-constraints model. Behav. Ecol. Sociobiol. 50: 329–338.

    Google Scholar 

  • Glander, K. E. (1992). Dispersal patterns in Costa Rican mantled howling monkeys. Int. J.Primatol. 13: 415–436.

    Google Scholar 

  • Harcourt, A. H. (1987). Dominance and fertility among female primates. J. Zool., Lond. 213: 471–487.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hemelrijk, C. (1999). An individual-oriented model of the emergence of despotic and egalitarian societies. Proc. R. Soc. Lond. B 266: 361–369.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hill, D. A., and Okayasu, N. (1995). Absence of "youngest ascendancy" in the dominance relations of sisters in wild Japanese macaques (Macaca fuscata yakui). Behaviour 132: 367–379.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hill, D. A., and Okayasu, N. (1996). Determinants of dominance among female macaques: Nepotism, demography and danger. In Fa, J. E., and Lindburg, D. G. (eds.), Evolution and Ecology of Macaque Societies, Cambridge University Press, Cambridge, pp. 459–472.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hinde, R. A. (1976). Interactions, relationships and social structure. Man 11: 1–17.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hladik, C. M. (1975). Ecology, diet and social patterns in Old andNewWorld primates. InTuttle, R. H. (ed.), Socioecology and Psychology of Primates, Mouton, The Hague, pp. 3–35.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hladik, C. M. (1977). A comparative study of the feeding strategies of two sympatric species of leaf monkeys: Presbytis senex and Presbytis entellus. In Clutton-Brock, T. H. (ed.), Primate Ecology: Studies of Feeding and Ranging Behaviour in Lemurs, Monkeys and Apes, Academic Press, London, pp. 323–353.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hrdy, S. B., and Hrdy, D. B. (1976). Hierarchical relations among female Hanuman langurs (Primates: Colobinae, Presbytis entellus). Science 193: 913–915.

    Google Scholar 

  • Huntingford, F., and Turner, A. K. (1987). Animal Conflict, Chapman and Hall, London.

    Google Scholar 

  • Isbell, L. A. (1991). Contest and scramble competition: Patterns of female aggression and ranging behavior among primates. Behav. Ecol. 2: 143–155.

    Google Scholar 

  • Isbell, L. A., and Pruetz, J. D. (1998). Differences between vervet (Cercopithecus aethiops) and patas monkeys (Erythrocebus patas) in agonistic interactions between adult females. Int.J. Primatol. 19: 837–855.

    Google Scholar 

  • Isbell, L. A., Pruetz, J. D., and Young, T. P. (1998). Movements of vervets (Cercopithecus aethiops) and patas monkeys (Erythrocebus patas) as estimators of food resource size, density, and distribution. Behav. Ecol. Sociobiol. 42: 123–133.

    Google Scholar 

  • Isbell, L. A., and van Vuren, D. (1996). Differential costs of locational and social dispersal and their consequences for female group-living primates. Behaviour 133: 1–36.

    Google Scholar 

  • Janson, C. H. (1985). Aggressive competition and individual food consumption in wild brown capuchin monkeys (Cebus apella). Behav. Ecol. Sociobiol. 18: 125–138.

    Google Scholar 

  • Janson, C. H. (1986). The mating system as determinant of social evolution in capuchin monkeys (Cebus). In Else, J.G., and Lee, P. C. (eds.), Primate Ecology and Conservation, Cambridge University Press, Cambridge, pp. 169–179.

    Google Scholar 

  • Janson, C. H. (1988a). Intra-specific food competition and primate social structure:Asynthesis.Behaviour 105: 1–17.

    Google Scholar 

  • Janson, C. H. (1988b). Food competition in brown capuchin monkeys (Cebus apella): Quantitative effects of group size and tree productivity. Behaviour 105: 53–76.

    Google Scholar 

  • Janson, C. H. (1992). Evolutionary ecology of primate social structure. In Smith, E. A., and Winterhalder, B. (eds.), Evolutionary Ecology and Human Behavior, Aldine de Gruyter, New York, pp. 95–130.

    Google Scholar 

  • Janson, C. H. (2000). Primate socio-ecology: The end of the golden age. Evol. Anthropol. 9: 73–86.

    Google Scholar 

  • Janson, C. H., and Goldsmith, M. L. (1995). Predicting group size in primates: Foraging costs and predation risks. Behav. Ecol. 6: 326–336.

    Google Scholar 

  • Janson, C. H., and van Schaik, C. P. (1988). Recognizing the many faces of primate food competition: Methods. Behaviour 105: 165–186.

    Google Scholar 

  • Jones, C. B. (1980). The function of status in the mantled howler monkey, Alouatta palliata GRAY: Intraspecific competition for group membership in a folivorous neotropical primate. Primates 21: 389–405.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kappeler, P.M. (1999). Lemur social structure and convergence in primate socioecology. In Lee, P. C. (ed.), Comparative Primate Socioecology, Cambridge University Press, Cambridge, pp. 273–299.

    Google Scholar 

  • Koenig, A. (2000). Competitive regimes in forest-dwelling Hanuman langur females (Semnopithecus entellus). Behav. Ecol. Sociobiol. 48: 93–109.

    Google Scholar 

  • Koenig, A., Beise, J., Chalise, M. K., and Ganzhorn, J. U. (1998). When females should contest for food—testing hypotheses about resource density, distribution, size, and quality with Hanuman langurs (Presbytis entellus). Behav. Ecol. Sociobiol. 42: 225–237.

    Google Scholar 

  • Koenig, A., and Borries, C. (2001). Socioecology ofHanumanlangurs—the story of their success.Evol. Anthropol. 10: 122–137.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lomnicki, A. (1988). Population Ecology of Individuals, Princeton University Press, Princeton.

    Google Scholar 

  • Maestripieri, D. (1996). Primate cognition and the bared-teeth display: A reevaluation of the concept of formal dominance. J. Comp. Psychol. 110: 402–405.

    Google Scholar 

  • Matsumura, S. (1998). Relaxed dominance relations among female moor macaques (Macaca maurus) in their natural habitat, South Sulawesi, Indonesia. Folia Primatol. 69: 346–356.

    Google Scholar 

  • Matsumura, S. (1999). The evolution of “egalitarian” and “despotic” social systems among macaques. Primates 40: 23–31.

    Google Scholar 

  • Matsumura, S., and Kobayashi, T. (1998).Agame model for dominance relations among groupliving animals. Behav. Ecol. Sociobiol. 42: 77–84.

    Google Scholar 

  • Maynard Smith, J. (1974). The theory of games and the evolution of animal conflicts. J. Theor.Biol. 47: 209–221.

    Google Scholar 

  • Mitchell, C. L., Boinski, S., and van Schaik, C. P. (1991). Competitive regimes and female bonding in two species of squirrel monkeys (Saimiri oerstedi and S. sciureus). Behav. Ecol.Sociobiol. 28: 55–60.

    Google Scholar 

  • Mori, A. (1979). Analysis of population changes by measurement of body weight in theKoshima troop of Japanese monkeys. Primates 20: 371–397.

    Google Scholar 

  • Nicholson, A. J. (1954). An outline of the dynamics of animal populations. Austral. J. Zool. 2: 9–65.

    Google Scholar 

  • Overdorff, D. J. (1996). Ecological correlates to social structure in two lemur species in Madagascar. Am. J. Phys. Anthropol. 100: 487–506.

    Google Scholar 

  • Parker, G. A. (1974). Assessment strategy and the evolution of fighting behaviour. J. Theor.Biol. 47: 223–243.

    Google Scholar 

  • Pope, T. R. (2000). Reproductive success increases with degree of kinship in cooperative coalitions of female red howler monkeys (Alouatta seniculus). Behav. Ecol. Sociobiol. 48: 253–267.

    Google Scholar 

  • Popp, J. L., and DeVore, I. (1979). Aggressive competition and social dominance theory: Synopsis. In Hamburg, D. A., and McCown, E. R. (eds.), The Great Apes, Benjamin-Cummings, Menlo Park, pp. 317–338.

    Google Scholar 

  • Preuschoft, S., and van Schaik, C. P. (2000). Dominance and communication. In Aureli, F., and de Waal, F. B. M. (eds.), Natural Conflict Resolution, University of California Press, Berkeley, pp. 77–105.

    Google Scholar 

  • Pruetz, J.D., and Isbell, L. A. (2000). Correlations of food distribution and patch size with agonistic interactions in female vervets (Chlorocebus aethiops) and patas monkeys (Erythrocebus patas) living in simple habitats. Behav. Ecol. Sociobiol. 49: 38–47.

    Google Scholar 

  • Pusey, A. E., and Packer, C. (1997). The ecology of relationships. In Krebs, J. R., and Davies, N. B. (eds.), Behavioral Ecology. An Evolutionary Approach, 4th ed., Blackwell, Oxford, pp. 254–283.

    Google Scholar 

  • Radespiel, U., Cepok, S., Zietemann, V., and Zimmermann, E. (1998). Sex-specific usage patterns of sleeping sites in grey mouse lemurs (Microcebus murinus) in northwestern Madagascar. Am. J. Primatol. 46: 77–84.

    Google Scholar 

  • Robinson, J.G. (1981). Spatial structure in foraging groups of wedge-capped capuchin monkeys Cebus nigrivittatus. Anim. Behav. 29: 1036–1056.

    Google Scholar 

  • Robinson, J. G. (1988). Group size in wedge-capped capuchin monkeys Cebus olivaceus and the reproductive success of males and females. Behav. Ecol. Sociobiol. 23: 187–197.

    Google Scholar 

  • Saito, C. (1996). Dominance and feeding success in female Japanese macaques, Macaca fuscata: Effects of food patch size and inter-patch distance. Anim. Behav. 51: 967–980.

    Google Scholar 

  • Saito, C., Sato, S., Suzuki, S., Sugiura, H., Agetsuma, N., Takahata, Y., Sasaki, C., Takahashi, H., Tanaka, T., and Yamagiwa, J. (1998). Aggressive intergroup encounters in two populations of Japanese macaques (Macaca fuscata). Primates 39: 303–312.

    Google Scholar 

  • Sherman, P.W. (1988). The level of analysis. Anim. Behav. 36: 616–619.

    Google Scholar 

  • Silk, J. B. (1993). The evolution of social conflict among female primates. In Mason, W. A., and Mendoza, S. P. (eds.), Primate Social Conflict, State University of New York Press, New York, pp. 49–83.

    Google Scholar 

  • Sommer, V., and Rajpurohit, L. S. (1989). Male reproductive success in harem troops of Hanuman langurs (Presbytis entellus). Int. J. Primatol. 10: 293–317.

    Google Scholar 

  • Stacey, P.B. (1986). Group size and foraging efficiency in yellow baboons. Behav. Ecol. Sociobiol. 18: 175–187.

    Google Scholar 

  • Steenbeek, R., and van Schaik, C. P. (2000). Competition and group size in Thomas's langurs (Presbytis thomasi): The folivore paradox revisited. Behav. Ecol. Sociobiol. 49: 100–110.

    Google Scholar 

  • Sterck, E. H. M. (1998). Female dispersal, social organization, and infanticide in langurs: Are they linked to human disturbance. Am. J. Primatol. 44: 235–254.

    Google Scholar 

  • Sterck, E. H. M. (1999).Variation in langur social organization in relation to the socioecological model, human habitat alteration, and phylogenetic constraints. Primates 40: 201–215.

    Google Scholar 

  • Sterck, E. H. M., and Steenbeek, R. (1997). Female dominance relationships and food competition in the sympatric Thomas langur and long-tailed macaque. Behaviour 134: 749–774.

    Google Scholar 

  • Sterck, E. H. M., Watts, D. P., and van Schaik, C. P. (1997). The evolution of female social relationships in nonhuman primates. Behav. Ecol. Sociobiol. 41: 291–309.

    Google Scholar 

  • Stevenson, P. R., and Castellanos, M. C. (2000). Feeding rates and daily path range of the Colombian woolly monkeys as evidence for between-and within-group competition. Folia Primatol. 71: 399–408.

    Google Scholar 

  • Suzuki, S., Noma, N., and Izawa, K. (1998). Inter-annual variation of reproductive parameters and fruit availability in two populations of Japanese macaques. Primates 39: 313–324.

    Google Scholar 

  • Strier, K. B. (1999). Why is female kin bonding so rare? Comparative sociality of neotropical primates. In Lee, P. C. (ed.), Comparative Primate Socioecology, Cambridge University Press, Cambridge, pp. 300–319.

    Google Scholar 

  • Terborgh, J., and Janson, C. H. (1986). The socioecology of primate groups. Ann. Rev. Ecol.Syst. 17: 111–135.

    Google Scholar 

  • Thierry, B. (1985). Patterns of agonistic interactions in three species of macaque (Macaca mulatta, M. fascicularis, M. tonkeana). Aggressive Behav. 11: 223–233.

    Google Scholar 

  • Thierry, B. (1990). Feedback loop between kinship and dominance: The macaque model.J. Theor. Biol. 145: 511–521.

    Google Scholar 

  • Thierry, B. (2000). Covariation of conflict management patterns across macaque species. In Aureli, F., and DeWaal, F.B. M. (eds.), Natural Conflict Resolution, University of California Press, Berkeley, pp. 106–128.

    Google Scholar 

  • Trivers, R. L. (1972). Parental investment and sexual selection. In Campbell, B. (ed.), Sexual Selection and the Descent of Man, Aldine, Chicago, pp. 136–179.

    Google Scholar 

  • van Hooff, J.A.R.A. M., and van Schaik, C. P. (1992). Cooperation in competition: The ecology of primate bonds. In Harcourt, A. H., and DeWaal, F. B. M. (eds.), Coalitions and Alliances in Humans and Other Animals, Oxford University Press, Oxford, pp. 357–389.

    Google Scholar 

  • van Noordwijk, M.A., and van Schaik, C.P. (1999).The effects of dominance rank and group size on female lifetime reproductive success in wild long-tailed macaques, Macaca fascicularis.Primates 40: 105–130.

    Google Scholar 

  • van Schaik, C. P. (1983).Why are diurnal primates living in groups? Behaviour 87: 120–144.

    Google Scholar 

  • van Schaik, C. P. (1989). The ecology of social relationships amongst female primates. In Standen, V., and Foley, R. A. (eds.), Comparative Socioecology. The Behavioural Ecology of Humans and Other Mammals, Blackwell, Oxford, pp. 195–218.

    Google Scholar 

  • van Schaik, C. P. (1996). Social evolution in primates: The role of ecological factors and male behaviour. Proc. Brit. Acad. 88: 9–31.

    Google Scholar 

  • van Schaik, C. P., and Aureli, F. (2000). The natural history of valuable relationships in primates.In Aureli, F., and De Waal, F. B. M. (eds.), Natural Conflict Resolution, University of California Press, Berkeley, pp. 307–333.

    Google Scholar 

  • van Schaik, C. P., and Kappeler, P. M. (1996). The social systems of gregarious lemurs: Lack of convergence with anthropoids due to evolutionary disequilibrium? Ethology 102: 915–941.

    Google Scholar 

  • van Schaik, C. P., and van Noordwijk, M. A. (1988). Scramble and contest in feeding competition among female long-tailed macaques (Macaca fascicularis). Behaviour 105: 77–98.

    Google Scholar 

  • Vehrencamp, S. (1983).Amodel for the evolution of despotic versus egalitarian societies. Anim.Behav. 31: 667–682.

    Google Scholar 

  • Waser, P. (1977). Feeding, ranging, and group size in the mangabey Cercocebus albigena. In Clutton-Brock, T. H. (ed.), Primate Ecology: Studies of Feeding and Ranging Behaviour in Lemurs, Monkeys and Apes, Academic Press, London, pp. 183–222.

    Google Scholar 

  • Watts, D. P. (1990). Ecology of gorillas and its relation to female transfer in mountain gorillas.Int. J. Primatol. 11: 21–45.

    Google Scholar 

  • Watts, D. P. (1994). Agonistic relationships between female mountain gorillas (Gorilla gorilla beringei). Behav. Ecol. Sociobiol. 34: 347–358.

    Google Scholar 

  • Watts, D. P. (1996). Comparative socio-ecology of gorillas. In McGrew, W. C., Marchant, L. F., and Nishida, T. (eds.), Great Ape Societies, Cambridge University Press, Cambridge, pp. 16–28.

    Google Scholar 

  • Watts, D. P., Colmenares, F., and Arnold, K. (2000). Redirection, consolation, and male policing.In Aureli, F., and De Waal, F. B. M. (eds.), Natural Conflict Resolution, University of California Press, Berkeley, pp. 281–301.

    Google Scholar 

  • Whitten, P. L. (1983). Diet and dominance among female vervet monkeys (Cercopithecus aethiops). Am. J. Primatol. 5: 139–159.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wrangham, R.W. (1979). On the evolution of ape social systems. Soc. Sci. Inf. 18: 335–368.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wrangham, R. W. (1980). An ecological model of female-bonded primate groups. Behaviour 75: 262–300.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wright, P. C. (1999). Lemur traits and Madagascar ecology: Coping with an island environment.Yearb. Phys. Anthropol. 42: 31–72.

    Google Scholar 

  • Zinner, D. (1999). Relationship between feeding time and food intake in hamadryas baboons (Papio hamadryas) and the value of feeding time as predictor of food intake. Zoo Biol. 18: 495–505.

    Google Scholar 

  • Zucker, E. L., and Clarke, M. R. (1998). Agonistic and affiliative relationships of adult female howlers (Alouatta palliata) in Costa Rica over a 4-year period. Int. J. Primatol. 19: 433–449.

    Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Cite this article

Koenig, A. Competition for Resources and Its Behavioral Consequences Among Female Primates. International Journal of Primatology 23, 759–783 (2002). https://doi.org/10.1023/A:1015524931226

Download citation

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1023/A:1015524931226

Navigation