Hostname: page-component-7c8c6479df-nwzlb Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-03-28T13:06:40.474Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Joint attention and social-emotional approach behavior in children with autism

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  04 March 2009

Peter Mundy*
Affiliation:
University of Miami
*
Peter Mundy, Department of Psychology, University of Miami, Coral Gables, FL 33124.

Abstract

Autism is a development disorder that is characterized by a significant disturbance of social development. Research strongly suggests that this disorder results from neurological anomalies or deficits. However, both the specific neural systems involved in autism, and the most pertinent behavioral functions of those systems remains unclear. One current topic of debate concerns the degree to which the social disturbance of autism may result from developmental anomalies in neurological systems that subserve cognitive, or affective processes. In this paper a model of the neurological, cognitive, and affective processes involved in the pathogenesis of autism will be described in the context of an attempt to understand dissociations in the early social-skill development of these children. Young children with autism are better able to use social-communication gestures to request objects or events than they are able to use similar gesture simply to initiate joint or socially shared attention relative to an object or event. An integration of recent research suggests that joint attention skill development differs from requesting skill development with regard to affective and cognitive processes that may be associated with frontal and midbrain neurological systems. In particular, this integration of the literature suggests the following: (a) there is a specific neurological subsystem that regulates and promotes what are called social-emotional approach behaviors; (b) the tendency to initiate joint attention bids is prototypical of a social-emotional approach behavior; and (c) attenuation of social-approach behaviors in children with autism leads to a specific impoverishment of social information processing opportunities. This impoverishment has a lifelong negative effect on the social cognitive development of these children.

Type
Articles
Copyright
Copyright © Cambridge University Press 1995

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Adamson, L., & Bakeman, R. (1985). Affect and attention: Infants observed with mothers and peers. Child Development, 56, 582593.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Adrien, J., Faure, M., Perrot, A., Hameury, L., Garreau, B., Barthelemy, C., & Sauvage, D. (1991). Autism and family home movies: Preliminary findings. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders. 21, 4349.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bachevalier, J. (1994). Medial temporal lobe structures and autism: A review of clinical and experimental findings. Neuropsychologia, 32, 627648.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bahrick, L., & Watson, J. (1985). Detection of intermodal proprioceptive-visual contingency as a potential basis of self perception in infancy. Developmental Psychology, 21, 963973.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Baron-Cohen, S. (1989a). The autistic child's theory of mind: A case of specific developmental delay. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 30, 285297.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Baron-Cohen, S. (1989b). Joint-attention deficits in autism: Toward a cognitive analysis. Development and Psychopathology, 3, 185190.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Baron-Cohen, S., Tager-Flusberg, H., & Cohen, D. (1993). Understanding other minds: Perspectives from Autism. Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Baron-Cohen, S., Spitz, A., & Cross, P. (1993). Do children with autism recognize surprise? A research note. Cognition and Emotion, 7, 507516.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bates, E., Benigni, L., Bretherton, I., Camaioni, L., & Volterra, V. (1979). The emergence of symbols: Cognition and communication in infancy. New York: Academic Press.Google Scholar
Bates, E., Camaioni, L., & Volterra, V. (1975). The acquisition of performatives prior to speech. Merrill-Palmer Quarterly, 21, 205224.Google Scholar
Bishop, D. (1993). Annotation: Autism, execution functions and theory of mind: A Neuropsychological Perspective. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 34, 279293.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bretherton, I. (1991). Intentional communication and the development of an understanding of mind. In Frye, D. & Moore, C. (Eds.), Children's theories of mind: Mental states and social understanding (pp. 271289). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Bruner, J. (1975). From communication to language: A psychological perspective. Cognition, 3, 255287.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bruner, J. (1981). Learning how to do things with words. In Bruner, J. & Garton, A. (Eds.), Human growth and development (pp. 6284). London: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Bruner, J., & Sherwood, V. (1983). Thought, language and interaction in infancy. In Call, J., Galenson, E., & Tyson, R. (Eds.), Frontiers of infant psychiatry (pp. 3855). New York: Basic Books.Google Scholar
Buchsbaum, M., Siegel, B., Wu, J., Hazlett, E., Sicotte, N., Haier, R., Tanguay, P., Asarnow, R., Cadorette, T., Donoghue, D., Lagunas-Solar, M., Lott, I., Paek, J., & Sabalesky, D. (1992). Brief report: Attention performance in autism and regional brain metabolic rate assessed by positron emission tomography. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 22, 115125.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Campos, J., & Stenberg, C. (1981). Perception, appraisal, and emotion. The onset of social referencing. In Lamb, M. & Sherrod, L. (Eds.), Infant social cognition (pp. 273314). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Caplan, R., Chugani, H., Messa, C., Guthrie, D., Sigman, M., Traversay, J., & Mundy, P. (1993). Hemispherectomy for early onset intractable seizures: Presurgical cerebral glucose metabolism and postsurgical nonverbal communication patterns. Developmental Medicine and Child Neurology, 35, 582592.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Capps, L., Sigman, M., & Mundy, P. (1994). Attachment security in children with autism. Development and Psychopathology, 6, 249261.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Cicchetti, D. (1984). The emergence of developmental psychopathology. Child Development, 55, 17.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cicchetti, D. (1993). Developmental psychopathology: Reactions, reflections and projections. Developmental Review, 13, 471502.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Cicchetti, D., & Pogge-Hesse, P. (1981). The relation between emotion and cognition in infant development. In Lamb, M. & Sherrod, L. (Eds.), Infant social cognition (pp. 205272). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Courchesne, E. (1989). Neuroanatomical systems involved in infantile autism: The implications of cerebral abnormalities. In Dawson, G. (Ed.), Autism (pp. 119143). New York: Guilford Press.Google ScholarPubMed
Curcio, F. (1978). Sensorimotor functioning and communication in mute autistic children. Journal of Autism and Childhood Schizophrenia, 8, 282292.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Davidson, R., Ekman, P., Saron, C., Senulis, J., & Friesen, W. (1990). Approach-withdrawal and cerebral asymmetry: Emotion, expression and brain physiology. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 58, 330341.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Davidson, R., & Fox, N. (1989). Frontal asymmetry predicts infants' response to maternal separation. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 98, 127131.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Damasio, A., & Maurer, R. (1978). A neurological model of childhood autism. Archives of Neurology, 35, 777786.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Dawson, G. (1991). A psychobiological perspective on the early socio-emotional development of children with autism. In Cicchetti, D. & Toth, S. (Eds.), Rochester symposium on developmental psychopathology, Volume 3. Rochester, NY: University of Rochester Press.Google Scholar
Dawson, G. (1994). Development of emotional expression and emotion regulation in infancy: Contribution of the frontal lobe. In Dawson, G. & Fischer, K. (Eds.), Human behavior and the developing brain (pp. 518536). New York: Guilford Press.Google Scholar
Dawson, G., & Adams, A. (1984). Imitation and social responsiveness in autistic children. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 12, 209225.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Dawson, G., Hill, D., Spencer, A., Galpert, L., & Wilson, L. (1990). Affective changes between young autistic children and their mothers. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 18, 335345.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Dawson, G., Klinger, G., Panagiotides, H., Hill, D., & Spieker, S. (1992). Frontal lobe activity and affective behavior of infants of mothers with depressive symptoms. Child Development, 63, 725737.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Fein, D., Humes, M., Kaplan, E., Lucci, D., & Waterhouse, L. (1984). The question of left hemisphere dysfunction in infantile autism. Psychological Bulletin, 95, 258281.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Fein, D., Pennington, B., & Waterhouse, L. (1987). Implications of social deficits in autism for neurological dysfunction. In Schopler, E. & Mesibov, G. (Eds.), Neurobiological issues in autism (pp. 127144). New York: Plenum Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Feldstein, S., Jaffe, J., Beebe, B., Crown, C., Jasnow, M., Fox, H., & Gordon, S. (1993). Coordinated interpersonal timing in adult-infant vocal interactions: A cross-site replication. Infant Behavior and Development, 16, 455470.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Field, T. (1979). Visual and cardiac responses to animate and inanimate faces by young preterm infants. Child Development, 50, 188194.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Fischer, K., & Bidell, T. (1991). Constraining nativist inferences about cognitive capacities. In Carey, S. & Gelman, R. (Eds.), The epigenesis of mind: Essays on biology and cognition (pp. 199235). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Fischer, K., & Pipp, S. (1984). Process in cognitive development: Optimal level and skill acquisition. In Sternberg, R. (Ed.), Mechanisms of cognitive development (pp. 4580). New York: W. H. Freeman.Google Scholar
Fotheringham, J. (1991). Autism: Its primary psychological and neurological deficit. Canadian Journal of Psychiatry, 36, 686692.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Fox, N. (1991). It's not left, it's right: Electroencephalograph asymmetry and the development of emotion. American Psychologist, 46, 863872.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Fox, N., & Davidson, R. (1987). Electroencephalogram asymmetry in response to the approach of a stranger and maternal separation in 10-month-old infants. Developmental Psychology, 23, 233240.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Fox, N., & Davidson, R. (1988). Patterns of brain electrical activity during facial signs of emotion in 10- month-old infants. Developmental Psychology, 24, 230236.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Frith, U. (1989). A new look at language and communication in autism. British Journal of Disorders of Communication, 24, 123150.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Goldman-Rakic, P. (1987). Circuitry of primate prefrontal cortex and regulation of behavior by representational knowledge. In Plum, F. & Mountcastel, V. (Eds.), Higher cortical fund ion: Handbook of physiology (pp. 447466). Washington, DC: American Physiology Society.Google Scholar
Gomez, C., Sarria, E., & Tamarit, J. (1993). The comparative study of early communication and theories of mind: Ontology, phylogeny, and pathology. In Baron-Cohen, S., Tager-Flusberg, H., & Cohen, D. (Eds.), Understanding other minds: Perspective from autism (pp. 397426). Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Gray, J. (1987). The psychology of fear and stress. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Hobson, P. (1993). Autism and the development of mind. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Hughes, C., & Russell, J. (1993). Autistic children's difficulty with metal disengagement from an object: Its implications for theories of autism. Developmental Psychology, 29, 498510.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Humphrey, N. (1976). The social function of intellect. In Bateson, P. & Hinde, R. (Eds.), Growing points in ethology (pp. 303317). London: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Izard, C. (1979). The maximally discriminate facial movement coding system. Newark: Instructional Resources Center, University of Delaware.Google Scholar
Jennings, K. (1975). People versus object orientation, social behavior, and intellectual abilities in preschool children. Developmental Psychology, 11, 511519.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Jones, S., Collins, K., & Hong, H. (1991). An audience effect on smile production in 10-month-old infants. Psychological Science, 2, 4549.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kagan, J., Resnick, J., & Snidman, N. (1988). Biological basis of childhood shyness. Science, 240, 167171.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kanner, L. (1943). Autistic disturbances of affective contact. Nervous Child, 2, 217250.Google Scholar
Karmiloff-Smith, A. (1992). Beyond modularity. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Kasari, C., Sigman, M., Mundy, P., & Yirmiya, N. (1990). Affective sharing in the context of joint attention interactions of normal, autistic, and mentally retarded children. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 20, 87100.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kasari, K., Sigman, M., & Yirmiya, N. (1993). Focused and social attention of autistic children in interactions with familiar and unfamiliar adults: A comparison of autistic, mentally retarded, and normal children. Development and Psychopathology, 5, 403414.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Landry, S., & Loveland, K. (1989). The effect of social context on the functional communication skills of autistic children. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 19, 283287.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
LeDoux, J. (1989). Cognitive-emotional interactions in the brain. Cognition and Emotion, 3, 267289.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Leekham, S., & Perner, J. (1991). Does the autistic child have a metarepresentational deficit? Cognition, 40, 203218.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Leslie, A. (1987). Pretense and representation: The origins of “theory of mind.” Psychological Review, 94, 412426.Google Scholar
Leslie, A., & Frith, U. (1990). Prospects for a cognitive neuropsychology of autism: Hobson's choice. Psychological Review, 97, 122131.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Leslie, A., & Happe, E. F. (1989). Autism and ostensive communication: The relevance of metarepresentation. Development and Psychopathology, 1, 205212.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Leslie, A., & Thaiss, L. (1992). Domain specificity in conceptual development: Neuropsychological evidence from autism. Cognition, 43, 225251.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lewis, M. (1993). Emotion-cognition interactions in early infant development. Cognition and Emotion, 7, 145170.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lewy, A., & Dawson, G. (1992). Social stimulation and joint attention deficits of young autistic children. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 20, 555566.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Loveland, K. (1991). Social affordances and interaction II: Autism and the affordances of the human environment. Ecological Psychology, 3, 99119.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Loveland, K., & Landry, S. (1986). Joint attention and language in autism and developmental language delay. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 16, 335349.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Maclean, P. (1985). Brain evolution relating to family, play, and the separation call. Archives of General Psychiatry, 42, 405417.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Malatesta, C., & Izard, C. (1984). The ontogenesis of human social signals: From biological imperative to symbolic utilization. In Fox, N. & Davidson, R. (Eds.), The psychology of affective development (pp. 161206). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
McEvoy, R., Rogers, S., & Pennington, R. (1993). Executive function and social communication deficits in young, autistic children. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 34, 563578.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Moore, C., & Corkum, V. (in press). Social understanding at the end of the first year of life. Developmental Review.Google Scholar
Mundy, P., & Hogan, A. (1994). Joint attention, intersubjectivity and autistic psychopathology. In Cicchetti, D. & Toth, S. (Eds.), Rochester symposium on developmental psychopathology: Disorder and dysfunction of the self (pp. 131). Rochester, NY: University of Rochester Press.Google Scholar
Mundy, P., & Sigman, M. (1989a). Specifying the nature of social impairment in autism. In Dawson, G. (Ed.), Autism: New perspectives on diagnosis, natures, and treatment (pp. 321). New York: Guilford Press.Google Scholar
Mundy, P., & Sigman, M. (1989b). The theoretical implications of joint attention deficits in autism. Development and Psychopathology, 1, 173183.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Mundy, P., Kasari, C., & Sigman, M. (1992). Nonverbal communication, affective sharing, and intersubjectivity. Infant Behavior and Development, 15, 377381.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Mundy, P., Sigman, M., & Kasari, C. (1990). A longitudinal study of joint attention and language development in autistic children. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 20, 115123.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Mundy, P., Sigman, M., & Kasari, C. (1993). The theory of mind and joint attention deficits in autism. In Baron-Cohen, S., Tager-Flusberg, H., & Cohen, D. (Eds.), Understanding other minds: Perspective from autism (pp. 181203). Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Mundy, P., Sigman, M., & Kasari, C. (1994). Joint attention, developmental level, and symptom presentation in young children with autism. Development and Psychopathology, 6, 389401.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Mundy, P., Sigman, M., Ungerer, J. A., & Sherman, T. (1986). Defining the social deficits in autism: The contribution of non-verbal communication measures. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 27, 657669.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Mundy, P., & Sheinkopf, S. (1993). Social behavior and the neurology of autism. International Pediatrics, 8, 205210.Google Scholar
Mundy, P., & Willoughby, J. (in press). Joint attention skills and social-emotional development in young children. In Lewis, M. (Ed.), Emotional development in atypical children. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Osterling, J., & Dawson, G. (in press). Early recognition of children with autism: A study of first birthday home video tapes. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disabilities.Google Scholar
Ozonoff, S. (in press). Executive function in autism. In Schopler, E. & Mesibov, G. (Eds.), Learning and cognition in autism. New York: Plenum Press.Google Scholar
Ozonoff, S., Pennington, B., & Rogers, S. (1991). Executive function deficits in high-functioning autistic individuals: Relationship to theory of mind. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 32, 10811105.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Panksepp, J., & Sahley, T. (1987). Possible brain opiod involvement in disrupted social intent and language development of autism. In Schopler, E. & Mesibov, G. (Eds.), Neurobiological issues in autism (pp. 357372). New York: Plenum Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Panksepp, J., Siving, S., & Normansell, L. (1985). Brain opiods and social emotions. In Reite, M. & Fields, T. (Eds.), The psychobiology of attachment and separation (pp. 349). New York: Academic Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Pennington, B. (in press). The working memory function of the prefrontal cortices: Implications from developmental and individual differences in cognition. In Haith, M., Benson, J., Roberts, R., & Pennington, B. (Eds.), Future-oriented processes in development. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.Google Scholar
Pribram, K. (1987). The subdivision of the frontal cortex revisited. In Perecman, E. (Ed.), The frontal lobes revisited (pp. 1139). New York: IRBN Press.Google Scholar
Rheingold, H., Hay, D., & West, M. (1976). Sharing in the second year of life. Child Development, 83, 898913.Google Scholar
Rogers, S., & Pennington, B. (1991). A theoretical approach to the deficits in infantile autism. Development and Psychopathology, 3, 137162.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Rumsey, J. (1985). Conceptual problem solving in highly verbal, nonretarded autistic men. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 15, 2336.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Scaife, M., & Bruner, J. (1975). The capacity for joint visual attention in the infant. Nature, 253, 265266.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Seibert, J. M., Hogan, A. E., & Mundy, P. C. (1982). Assessing interactional competencies: The Early Social-Communication Scales. Infant Mental Health Journal, 3, 244245.3.0.CO;2-R>CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Shapiro, R., Sherman, M., Clamari, G., & Koch, D. (1987). Attachment in autism and other developmental disorders. Journal of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, 26, 485490.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Sherrod, L. (1979). Social cognition in infancy: Attention to the human face. Infant Behavior and Development, 2, 279294.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Sigman, M. (1989). The application of developmental knowledge to a clinical problem: The study of childhood autism. In Cicchetti, D. (Ed.), The emergence of a discipline: Rochester symposium on developmental psychopathology (Vol. 1, pp. 165189). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Sigman, M., & Ungerer, J. A. (1984). Cognitive and language skills in autistic, mentally retarded, and normal children. Developmental Psychology, 20, 293302.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Sigman, M., & Mundy, P. (1989). Social attachments in autistic children. Journal of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, 28, 7481.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Sigman, M., Mundy, P., Sherman, T., & Ungerer, J. A. (1986). Social interactions of autistic, mentally retarded and normal children with their caregivers. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 27, 647656.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Snow, M., Hertzig, M., & Shapiro, T. (1987). Expressions of emotion in young autistic children. Journal of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, 26, 836838.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Sroufe, L., & Rutter, M. (1984). The domain of developmental psychopathology. Child Development, 55, 1729.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Stern, D. (1985). The interpersonal world of the infant. New York: Basic Books.Google Scholar
Strauss, E., Hunter, M., & Wada, J. (1993). Wisconsin card sorting performance: Effects of age of onset of damage and laterality of dysfunction. Journal of Clinical & Experimental Neuropsychology, 15, 896902.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Stuss, D., & Benson, D. F. (1984). Neuropsychological studies of the frontal lobes. Psychological Bulletin, 95, 328.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Suomi, S. (1981). The perception of contingency and social development. In Lamb, M. & Sherrod, L. (Eds.), Infant social cognition (pp. 177201). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Tomasello, M. (in press). Joint attention as social cognition. In Moore, C. & Dunham, P. (Eds.), Joint attention: Its origins and role in development. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Tronick, E. (1989). Emotions and emotional communication in infants. American Psychologist, 44, 112119.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Trevarthen, C. (1980). The foundations of intersubjectivity: Development of interpersonal and cooperative understanding. In Olson, D. (Ed.), The social foundation of language and thought: Essays in the honor of Jerome Bruner (pp. 316341). New York: Norton.Google Scholar
Trevarthen, C. (1990). Growth and education of the hemispheres. In Trevarthen, C. (Ed.), Brain circuits and functions of mind: Essays in the honor of Roger Sperry (pp. 334362). New York: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Tucker, D. (1992). Developing emotions and cortical networks. In Gunnar, M. & Nelson, C. (Eds.), Minnesota symposia on child psychology, Vol. 24, Developmental behavioral neuroscience (pp. 75128). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Volkmar, F., & Anderson, G. (1989). Neurochemical perspectives in infantile autism. In Dawson, G. (Ed.), Autism (pp. 208224). New York: Guilford Press.Google ScholarPubMed
Volkmar, F., & Cohen, D. (1988). Issues in the diagnosis and classification of infantile autism. In Lahey, B. & Kazdin, A. (Eds.), Advances in clinical child psychology (pp. 135). New York: Plenum Press.Google Scholar
Volkmar, F., Cohen, D., Bregman, J., Hooks, M., & Stevenson, J. (1989). An examination of social typologies in autism. Journal of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, 28, 8286.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Werner, H., & Kaplan, S. (1963). Symbol formation. New York: Wiley.Google Scholar
Wetherby, A. M., & Prutting, C. A. (1984). Profiles of communicative and cognitive-social abilities in autistic children. Journal of Speech and Hearing Research, 27, 367377.Google ScholarPubMed
Wetherby, A., Yonclas, D., & Bryan, A. (1989). Communicative profiles of handicapped preschool children: Implications for early identification. Journal of Speech and Hearing Disorders, 54, 148158.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
White, R. (1959). Motivation reconsidered: The concept of competence. Psychological Review, 66, 297333.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Wing, L., & Gould, J. (1979). Severe impairments of social interaction and associated abnormalities in children. Epidemiology and classification. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 9, 1129.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Yirmiya, N., Sigman, M., Kasari, C., & Mundy, P. (1992). Empathy and cognition in high-functioning children with autism. Child Development, 63, 150160.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed