Hostname: page-component-7c8c6479df-94d59 Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-03-28T08:40:15.609Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Young children fail to fully generalize a novel argument structure construction when exposed to the same input as older learners

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  22 June 2011

JEREMY K. BOYD*
Affiliation:
University of Illinois at Urbana-Champaign
ADELE E. GOLDBERG*
Affiliation:
Princeton University
*
Addresses for correspondence: Jeremy K. Boyd, Beckman Institute for Advanced Science and Technology, University of Illinois at Urbana-Champaign, 405 N. Mathews Ave., Urbana, IL 61801. e-mail: jkboyd@illinois.edu;
Adele E. Goldberg, Department of Psychology, Green Hall, Princeton University, Princeton, NJ 08544. e-mail: adele@princeton.edu.

Abstract

The present study1 exposed five-year-olds (M=5 ; 2), seven-year-olds (M=7 ; 6) and adults (M=22 ; 4) to instances of a novel phrasal construction, then used a forced choice comprehension task to evaluate their learning of the construction. The abstractness of participants' acquired representations of the novel construction was evaluated by varying the degree of lexical overlap that test items had with exposure items. We found that both child groups were less proficient than adults, but seven-year-olds showed evidence of across-the-board generalization whereas five-year-olds were sensitive to lexical overlap at test. This outcome is consistent with more conservative, item-based learning of syntactic patterns in younger children. Additionally, unlike adults and seven-year-olds, five-year-olds showed no evidence of having mastered the novel construction's linking rules. Thus, younger learners are less likely to generalize abstract argument structure constructions when exposed to the same systematic input as older learners.

Type
Articles
Copyright
Copyright © Cambridge University Press 2011

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

REFERENCES

Abbot-Smith, K., Lieven, E. & Tomasello, M. (2008). Graded representations in the acquisition of English and German transitive constructions. Cognitive Development 23, 4866.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Akhtar, N. & Tomasello, M. (1997). Young children's productivity with word order and verb morphology. Developmental Psychology 33, 952–65.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Baayen, R. H. (2008). Analyzing linguistic data: A practical introduction to statistics using R. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Baker, C. L. (1979). Syntactic theory and the projection problem. Linguistic Inquiry 10, 533–81.Google Scholar
Bates, E. & MacWhinney, B. (1982). Functionalist approaches to grammar. In Wanner, E. & Gleitman, L. R. (eds), Language acquisition: The state of the art, 175218. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Bencini, G. M. L. & Valian, V. V. (2008). Abstract sentence representations in 3-year-olds: Evidence from language production and comprehension. Journal of Memory and Language 59, 97–113.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bornstein, R. F. & D'Agostino, P. R. (1994). The attribution and discounting of perceptual fluency: Preliminary tests of a perceptual fluency/attribution model of the mere exposure effect. Social Cognition 12, 103–28.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Boyd, J. K., Gottschalk, E. A. & Goldberg, A. E. (2009). Linking rule acquisition in novel phrasal constructions. Language Learning 59, 6489.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Braine, M. D. S. (1976). Children's first word combinations. Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development 41, 1104.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Brainerd, C. J. & Mojardin, A. (1998). Children's and adults' spontaneous false memories: Long-term persistence and mere testing effects. Child Development 69, 1361–77.Google ScholarPubMed
Brainerd, C. J. & Reyna, V. F. (2004). Fuzzy-trace theory and memory development. Developmental Review 24, 396439.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Brainerd, C. J., Reyna, V. F. & Ceci, S. J. (2008). Developmental reversals in false memory: A review of data and theory. Psychological Bulletin 134, 343–82.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Brainerd, C. J., Reyna, V. & Forrest, T. (2002). Are young children susceptible to the false-memory illusion? Child Development 73, 1363–77.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Brooks, P. J. & Tomasello, M. (1999). How children constrain their argument structure constructions. Language 75, 720–38.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Casasola, M. (2005). When less is more: How infants learn to form an abstract categorical representation of support. Child Development 76, 279–90.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Casenhiser, D. & Goldberg, A. E. (2005). Fast mapping between a phrasal form and meaning. Developmental Science 8, 500508.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Culicover, Peter W. 1999. Syntactic nuts: Hard cases, syntactic theory and language acquisition reviewed by John R. Taylor, Cognitive Linguistics 10, 251–61.Google Scholar
Dąbrowska, E. & Szczerbiński, M. (2006). Polish children's productivity with case marking: The role of regularity, type frequency and phonological diversity. Journal of Child Language 33, 559–97.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Dąbrowska, E. & Tomasello, M. (2008). Rapid learning of an abstract language-specific category: Polish children's acquisition of the instrumental construction. Journal of Child Language 35, 533–58.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Dittmar, M., Abbot-Smith, K., Lieven, E. & Tomasello, M. (2008). Young German children's early syntactic competence: A preferential looking study. Developmental Science 11, 575–82.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Fernandes, K. J., Marcus, G. F., Di Nubila, J. A. & Vouloumanos, A. (2006). From semantics to syntax and back again: Argument structure in the third year of life. Cognition 100, B10B20.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Fisher, A. V. & Sloutsky, V. M. (2005). When induction meets memory: Evidence for gradual transition from similarity-based to category-based induction. Child Development 76, 583–97.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gentner, D. & Medina, J. (1998). Similarity and the development of rules. Cognition 65, 263–97.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gertner, Y., Fisher, C. & Eisengart, J. (2006). Learning words and rules: Abstract knowledge of word order in early sentence comprehension. Psychological Science 17, 684–91.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Goldberg, A. E. (2006). Constructions at work: The nature of generalization in language. Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Goldberg, A. E., Casenhiser, D. M. & Sethuraman, N. (2004). Learning argument structure generalizations. Cognitive Linguistics 15, 289316.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Goldberg, A. E., Casenhiser, D. M. & White, T. (2007). Constructions as categories of language. New Ideas in Psychology 25, 7086.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hudson Kam, C. L. & Newport, E. L. (2005). Regularizing unpredictable variation: The roles of adult and child learners in language formation and change. Language Learning and Development 1, 151–95.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hudson Kam, C. L. & Newport, E. L. (2009). Getting it right by getting it wrong: When learners change languages. Cognitive Psychology 59, 3066.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ingram, D. & Thompson, W. (1996). Early syntactic acquisition in German: Evidence for the Modal Hypothesis. Language 72, 97–120.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Jaeger, T. F. (2008). Categorical data analysis: Away from ANOVAs (transformation or not) and towards logit mixed models. Journal of Memory and Language 59, 434–46.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lakoff, George. (1970). Irregularity in syntax. New York: Holt Rinehart and Winston.Google Scholar
Lieven, E. V. M., Pine, J. M. & Baldwin, G. (1997). Lexically-based learning and early grammatical development. Journal of Child Language 24, 187219.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
MacWhinney, B. (1982). Basic syntactic processes. In Kuczaj, S. (ed.), Language development, vol. 1: Syntax and semantics. Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Maguire, M. J., Hirsh-Pasek, K., Golinkoff, R. M. & Brandone, A. C. (2008). Focusing on the relation: Fewer exemplars facilitate children's initial verb learning and extension. Developmental Science 11, 628–34.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Marchman, V. A. & Bates, E. (1994). Continuity in lexical and morphological development: A test of the critical mass hypothesis. Journal of Child Language 21, 339–66.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Munakata, Y. (2001). Graded representations in behavioral dissociations. Trends in Cognitive Sciences 5, 309315.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Munakata, Y., McClelland, J. L., Johnson, M. H. & Siegler, R. S. (1997). Rethinking infant knowledge: Toward an adaptive process account of successes and failures in object permanence tasks. Psychological Review 104, 686713.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Myers, J. L. (1976). Probability learning and sequence learning. In Estes, W. K. (ed.), Handbook of learning and cognitive processes, 171205. Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.Google Scholar
Pinker, S. (1989). Learnability and Cognition: The Acquisition of Argument Structure. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press/Bradford Books.Google Scholar
Quené, H. & van den Bergh, H. (2008). Examples of mixed-effects modeling with crossed random effects and with binomial data. Journal of Memory and Language 59, 413–25.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
R Development Core Team (2010). R: A language and environment for statistical computing. Vienna: R Foundation for Statistical Computing.Google Scholar
Ramage, A., Bayles, K., Helm-Estabrooks, N. & Cruz, R. (1999). Frequency of perseveration in normal subjects. Brain and Language 66, 329–40.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rovee-Collier, C. (1997). Dissociations in infant memory: Rethinking the development of implicit and explicit memory. Psychological Review 104, 467–98.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Savage, C., Lieven, E., Theakston, A. & Tomasello, M. (2003). Testing the abstractness of children's linguistic representations: Lexical and structural priming of syntactic constructions in young children. Developmental Science 6, 557–67.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Schlesinger, I. M. (1982). Steps to language: Toward a theory of language acquisition. Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Singleton, J. L. & Newport, E. L. (2004). When learners surpass their models: The acquisition of American Sign Language from inconsistent input. Cognitive Psychology 49, 370407.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Snow, C. E. & Hoefnagel-Höhle, M. (1978). The critical period for language acquisition: Evidence from second language learning. Child Development 49, 1114–28.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Strobl, C., Malley, J. & Tutz, G. (2009). An introduction to recursive partitioning: Rationale, application, and characteristics of classification and regression trees, bagging, and random forests. Psychological Methods 14, 323–48.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Tighe, T. J., Tighe, L. S. & Schechter, J. (1975). Memory for instances and categories in children and adults. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology 20, 2237.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Tomasello, M. (1992). First verbs: A case study of early grammatical development. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Tomasello, M. (2000). Do young children have adult syntactic competence? Cognition 74, 209–53.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Tomasello, M. (2003). Constructing a language: A usage-based theory of language acquisition. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
Williams, Edwin (1994). Remarks on lexical knowledge. Lingua 92, 7–34.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Wray, Alison (2002). The transition to language. New York: Oxford University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar