Hostname: page-component-8448b6f56d-c4f8m Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-04-23T06:08:17.978Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Morphological awareness and vocabulary acquisition: A longitudinal examination of their relationship in English-speaking children

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  16 May 2013

ERIN SPARKS*
Affiliation:
Dalhousie University
S. HÉLÈNE DEACON
Affiliation:
Dalhousie University
*
ADDRESS FOR CORRESPONDENCE Erin Sparks, Department of Psychology, Life Sciences Centre, Dalhousie University, Halifax, NS B3H 4R2, Canada. E-mail: erin.sparks@dal.ca

Abstract

Although a relationship between morphological awareness and vocabulary has been widely observed, questions remain about the direction of that relationship. This longitudinal study explores the temporal relationship between morphological awareness and vocabulary among monolingual English-speaking children. Participants were 100 children tested in Grades 2 and 3. We evaluated morphological awareness and vocabulary in both grades, along with phonological awareness, word reading, pseudoword reading, and nonverbal reasoning. Cross-lagged regression analyses with autoregressive controls assessed the temporal relationship between morphological awareness and vocabulary; morphological awareness at Grade 2 predicted change in vocabulary between Grades 2 and 3, but vocabulary did not predict change in morphological awareness. The results add to our understanding of the relationship between these two developing skills.

Type
Articles
Copyright
Copyright © Cambridge University Press 2013 

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

REFERENCES

Anglin, J. M. (1993). Vocabulary development: A morphological analysis. Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development, 58 (Serial No. 238).CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Baumann, J. F., Edwards, E. C., Boland, E. M., Olejnik, S., & Kame'enui, E. J. (2003). Vocabulary tricks: Effects of instruction in morphology and context on fifth-grade students’ ability to derive and infer word meanings. American Educational Research Journal, 40, 447494.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Baumann, J. F., & Kame'enui, E. J. (1991). Research on vocabulary instruction: Ode to Voltaire. In Flood, J., Lapp, J. J. D., & Squire, J. R. (Eds.), Handbook of research on teaching the English language arts (pp. 604632). New York: Macmillan.Google Scholar
Berko, J. (1958). The child's learning of English morphology. Word, 14, 150177.Google Scholar
Bowerman, B. L., & O'Connell, R. T. (1990). Linear statistical models: An applied approach (2nd ed.). Belmont, CA: Duxbury.Google Scholar
Bowers, P. N., & Kirby, J. R. (2010). Effects of morphological instruction on vocabulary acquisition. Reading and Writing, 23, 515537.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bowers, P. N., Kirby, J. R., & Deacon, S. H. (2010). The effects of morphological instruction on literacy skills: A systematic review of the literature. Review of Educational Research, 80, 144179.Google Scholar
Bowey, J. A. (2001). Nonword repetition and young children's receptive vocabulary: A longitudinal study. Applied Psycholinguistics, 22, 441469.Google Scholar
Carlisle, J. F. (1988). Knowledge of derivational morphology and spelling ability in fourth, sixth, and eighth graders. Applied Psycholinguistics, 9, 247266.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Carlisle, J. F. (1995). Morphological awareness and early reading achievement. In Feldman, L. B. (Ed.), Morphological aspects of language processing (pp. 189209). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Carlisle, J. F. (2000). Awareness of the structure and meaning of morphologically complex words: Impact on reading. Reading and Writing, 12, 169190.Google Scholar
Carlisle, J. F. (2007). Fostering morphological processing, vocabulary development, and reading comprehension. In Wagner, R., Muse, A., & Tannenbaum, K. (Eds.), Vocabulary acquisition: Implications for reading comprehension. New York: Guilford Press.Google Scholar
Carlisle, J. F., & Nomanbhoy, D. (1993). Phonological and morphological awareness in first graders. Applied Psycholinguistics, 14, 177195.Google Scholar
Cohen, J. (1988). Statistical power analysis for the behavioral sciences (2nd ed.). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Davis, C., & Bryant, P. (2006). Causal connections in the acquisition of an orthographic rule: A test of Uta Frith's developmental hypothesis. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 47, 849856.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
DeVellis, R. F. (2003). Scale development: Theory and applications (2nd ed.) Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage.Google Scholar
Dunn, L. M., & Dunn, L. M. (1997). Peabody Picture Vocabulary Test—Third edition: Manual. Circle Pines, MN: American Guidance Services.Google Scholar
Fowler, A., Feldman, L. B., Andjelkovic, D., & Oney, B. (2003). Morphological and phonological analysis by beginning readers: Evidence from Serbian and Turkish. In Assink, E. & Santa, D. (Eds.), Reading complex words: Cross-language studies (pp. 5379). Dordrecht: Kluwer.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Fowler, A. E., & Liberman, I. Y. (1995). The role of phonology and orthography in morphological awareness. In Feldman, L. B. (Ed.), Morphological aspects of language processing (pp. 157188). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Gathercole, S. E., Willis, C. S., Emslie, H., & Baddeley, A. D. (1992). Phonological memory and vocabulary development during the early school years: A longitudinal study. Developmental Psychology, 28, 887898.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Goswami, U. (1995). Transitive relational mappings in three- and four-year-olds: The analogy of Goldilocks and the Three Bears. Child Development, 66, 877892.Google Scholar
Hollingshead, A. B. (1957). Two Factor Index of Social Position. Unpublished manuscript, Yale University.Google Scholar
Hoosain, R. (1992). Psychological reality of the word in Chinese. In Chen, H. C. & Tzeng, O. J. I. (Eds.), Language processing in Chinese. Amsterdam: North-Holland.Google Scholar
Kieffer, M. J., & Lesaux, N. K. (2012). Development of morphological awareness and vocabulary knowledge in Spanish-speaking language minority learners: A parallel process latent growth curve model. Applied Psycholinguistics, 33, 2354.Google Scholar
Kirby, J. R., Deacon, S. H., Bowers, P. N., Izenberg, L., Wade-Woolley, L., & Parrila, R. (2012). Children's morphological awareness and reading ability. Reading and Writing, 25, 389410.Google Scholar
Ku, Y.-M., & Anderson, R. C. (2003). Development of morphological awareness in Chinese and English. Reading and Writing, 16, 399422.Google Scholar
Lewis, D. J., & Windsor, J. (1996). Children's analysis of derivational suffix meanings. Journal of Speech and Hearing Research, 39, 209216.Google Scholar
Mahony, D. (1994). Using sensitivity to word structure to explain variance in high school and college level reading ability. Reading and Writing, 15, 261294.Google Scholar
McBride-Chang, C., Tardif, T., Cho, J.-R., Shu, H., Fletcher, P., Stokes, S. F., et al. (2008). What's in a word? Morphological awareness and vocabulary knowledge in three languages. Applied Psycholinguistics, 29, 437462.Google Scholar
McBride-Chang, C., Wagner, R. K., Muse, A., Chow, B. W.-Y., & Shu, H. (2005). The role of morphological awareness in children's vocabulary acquisition in English. Applied Psycholinguistics, 26, 415435.Google Scholar
Metsala, J. L. (1999). Young children's phonological awareness and nonword repetition as a function of vocabulary development. Journal of Educational Psychology, 91, 319.Google Scholar
Muse, A. E. (2005). The nature of morphological knowledge. Unpublished doctoral dissertation, Florida State University.Google Scholar
Myers, R. (1990). Classical and modern regression with applications (2nd ed.). Boston: Duxbury.Google Scholar
Nagy, W. E. (2007). Metalinguistic awareness and the vocabulary–comprehension connection. In Wagner, R., Muse, A., & Tannenbaum, K. (Eds.), Vocabulary acquisition: Implications for reading comprehension. New York: Guilford Press.Google Scholar
Nagy, W. E., & Anderson, R. C. (1984). The number of words in printed school English. Reading Research Quarterly, 19, 304330.Google Scholar
Nagy, W. E., Berninger, V. W., & Abbott, R. D. (2006). Contributions of morphology beyond phonology to literacy outcomes of upper elementary and middle-school students. Journal of Educational Psychology, 98, 134147.Google Scholar
Nagy, W., Berninger, V., Abbott, R., Vaughan, K., & Vermeulen, K. (2003). Relationship of morphology and other language skills to literacy skills in at-risk second-grade readers and at-risk fourth-grade writers. Journal of Educational Psychology, 95, 730742.Google Scholar
Nicoladis, E., Palmer, A., & Marentette, P. (2007). The role of type and token frequency in using past tense morphemes correctly. Developmental Science, 10, 237254.Google Scholar
Nunes, T., Bryant, P., & Bindman, M. (1997). Morphological spelling strategies: Developmental stages and processes. Developmental Psychology, 33, 637649.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Pasquarella, A., Chen, X., Lam, K., Luo, Y. C., & Ramirez, G. (2011). Cross-language transfer of morphological awareness in Chinese–English bilinguals. Journal of Research in Reading, 34, 2342.Google Scholar
Penno, J. F., Wilkinson, I. A. G., & Moore, D. W. (2002). Vocabulary acquisition from teacher explanation and repeated listening to stories: Do they overcome the Matthew effect? Journal of Educational Psychology, 94, 2333.Google Scholar
Rosner, J., & Simon, D. (1971). The auditory analysis test: An initial report. Journal of Learning Disabilities, 4, 4048.Google Scholar
Ruffman, T., Slade, L., & Crowe, E. (2002). The relation between children's and mothers’ mental state language and theory-of-mind understanding. Child Development, 73, 734751.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Singson, M., Mahony, D., & Mann, V. (2000). The relation between reading ability and morphological skills: Evidence from derivational suffixes. Reading and Writing, 12, 219252.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Stahl, S. A., & Fairbanks, M. M. (1986). The effects of vocabulary instruction: A model-based meta-analysis. Review of Educational Research, 56, 72110.Google Scholar
Statistics Canada. (2007). Population Groups (28) and Sex (3) for the Population of Canada, Provinces, Territories, Census Metropolitan Areas and Census Agglomerations, 2006 Census. 20% Sample Data. 2006 Census of Population. Statistics Canada No. 97-562-XCB2006007. Retrieved May 1, 2012, from http://www12.statcan.gc.ca/census-recensement/2006/dp-pd/tbt/rp-eng.cfm?tabid=1&lang=e&apath=3&detail=0&dim=0&fl=a&free=0&gc=0&gid=837937&gk=0&grp=1&pid=92334&prid=0&ptype=88971,97154&s=0&showall=0&sub=0&temporal=2006&theme=80&vid=0&vnamee=&vnamef=&d1=0&d2=0&d3=0&d4=0&d5=0&d6=0Google Scholar
Tabachnick, B. C., & Fidell, L. S. (2007). Using multivariate statistics (4th ed.). Needham Heights, MA: Allyn & Bacon.Google Scholar
Tyler, A., & Nagy, W. E. (1989). The acquisition of English derivational morphology. Journal of Memory and Language, 28, 649667.Google Scholar
Wagner, R. K., Muse, A. E., & Tannenbaum, K. (2007). Promising avenues for better understanding implications of vocabulary development for reading comprehension. In Wagner, R., Muse, A., & Tannenbaum, K. (Eds.), Vocabulary acquisition: Implications for reading comprehension. New York: Guilford Press.Google Scholar
Wagner, R. K., Torgesen, J. K., Rashotte, C. A., Hecht, S. A., Barker, T. A., Burgess, S. R., et al. (1997). Changing relations between phonological processing abilities and word-level reading as children develop from beginning to skilled readers: A 5-year longitudinal study. Developmental Psychology, 33, 468479.Google Scholar
Wang, M., Yang, C., & Cheng, C. (2009). The contributions of phonology, orthography, and morphology in Chinese–English biliteracy acquisition. Applied Psycholinguistics, 30, 291314.Google Scholar
Wechsler, D. (1999). Wechsler Abbreviated Scale of Intelligence (WASI). San Antonio, TX: Psychological Corporation.Google Scholar
Woodcock, R. (1998). Woodcock Reading Mastery Tests—Revised. Circle Pines, MN: American Guidance Services.Google Scholar
Wysocki, K., & Jenkins, J. R. (1987). Deriving word meanings through morphological generalization. Reading Research Quarterly, 22, 6681.Google Scholar