Hostname: page-component-8448b6f56d-t5pn6 Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-04-23T20:08:52.365Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Phonological awareness and literacy skills in Korean: An examination of the unique role of body-coda units

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  05 January 2007

YOUNG-SUK KIM
Affiliation:
Harvard Graduate School of Education

Abstract

This study examines a salient intrasyllabic phonological unit in Korean, the body-coda unit, its role in literacy skills in Korean, and a possible source of the salience of body-coda units in the spoken language. Data were collected from Korean-speaking, monolingual beginning readers (41 kindergarteners, 40 first graders). The results indicate that body-coda boundary (e.g., ca-t) is more salient than onset–rime boundary (e.g., c-at) for Korean children and show that children's body-coda awareness is an important predictor of word decoding and spelling in Korean. Furthermore, the analysis of phonological neighbors and frequency of syllable types suggests that a phonotactic feature in Korean, the frequency of consonant–verb syllable type, may be a possible source of the saliency of the body-coda intrasyllabic division for Korean children.

Type
Articles
Copyright
© 2007 Cambridge University Press

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Aheeaegae Hangul onje gahruchilgga? [When should we start teaching Hangul?]. (2002, December 26). The Jungang Daily.
Aidinis A., & Nunes T. 2001. The role of different levels of PA in the development of reading and spelling in Greek. Reading and Writing: An Interdisciplinary Journal, 14, 145177.Google Scholar
Anthony J. L., & Lonigan C. J. 2004. The nature of phonological awareness: Converging evidence from four studies of preschool and early grade school children. Journal of Educational Psychology, 96, 4355.Google Scholar
Bond G. L., & Dykstra R. 1997. The cooperative research program in first-grade reading instruction. Reading Research Quarterly, 32, 348427.Google Scholar
Bryant P. E., Bradley L., MacLean M., & Crossland J. 1989. Nursery rhymes, phonological skills and reading. Journal of Child Language, 16, 407428.Google Scholar
Bryant P. E., MacLean M., & Bradley L. 1990. Rhyme, language, and children's reading. Applied Psycholinguistics, 11, 237252.Google Scholar
Bryant P. E., Maclean M., Bradley L., & Crossland J. 1990. Rhyme, alliteration, phoneme detection and learning to read. Developmental Psychology, 26, 429438.Google Scholar
Byrne B., & Fielding-Barnsley R. 1993. Evaluation of a program to teach phonemic awareness to young children. Journal of Educational Psychology, 85, 104111.Google Scholar
Carver R. P. 2003. The highly lawful relationships among pseudoword decoding, word identification, spelling, listening, and reading. Scientific Studies of Reading, 7, 127154.Google Scholar
Chall J. S. 1967. Learning to read: The great debate. New York: McGraw–Hill.
Cheun S.-B. 1980. Lapsus linguaeuy umwunloncek haysek [A phonological interpretation of lapsus linguae]. En, 5, 1532.Google Scholar
Cho J.-R., & McBride-Chang C. 2005. Correlates of Korean Hangul Acquisition among kindergartners and second graders. Scientific Studies of Reading, 9, 316.Google Scholar
De Cara B., & Goswami U. 2002. Similarity relations among spoken words: The special status of rimes in English. Behavior Research Methods, Instruments, and Computers, 34, 416423.Google Scholar
De Cara B., & Goswami U. 2003. Phonological neighborhourhood density: Effects in a rhyme awareness task in five-year-old children. Journal of Child Language, 30, 695710.Google Scholar
Derwing B. L., Yoon Y.-B., & Cho S.-H. 1990. The organization of the Korean syllable: Experimental evidence. In P. M. Clancy (Ed.), Japanese/Korean Linguistics (Vol. 2). Stanford, CA: Stanford Linguistics Association.
Dixon M., Stuart M., & Masterson J. 2002. The relationship between phonological awareness and the development of orthographic representations. Reading and Writing: An Interdisciplinary Journal, 15, 295316.Google Scholar
Duncan G. L., Seymour P. H. K., & Hill S. 1997. How important are rhyme and analogy in beginning reading? Cognition, 63, 171208.Google Scholar
Durgunoglu A. Y., Nagy W. E., & Hancin-Bhatt B. J. 1993. Cross-language transfer of PA. Journal of Educational Psychology, 85, 453465.Google Scholar
Durgunoglu A. Y., & Oney B. 1999. A cross-linguistic comparison of phonological awareness and word recognition. Reading and Writing, 11, 281299.Google Scholar
Frith U., Wimmer H., & Landerl K. 1998. Differences in phonological recoding in German- and English-speaking children. Scientific Studies of Reading, 2, 3154.Google Scholar
Frost J. 2001. Differences in reading development among Danish beginning-readers with high versus low phonemic awareness on entering grade one. Reading and Writing: An Interdisciplinary Journal, 14, 615642.Google Scholar
Gierut J. A., Morrisette M. L., & Champion A. H. 1999. Lexical constraints in phonological acquisition. Journal of Child Language, 26, 261294.Google Scholar
Gim C.-G. 1987. A study on syllable structure and some processes in its nucleus in Korean. Mahl, 12, 2569.Google Scholar
Goswami U. 1986. Children's use of analogy in learning to read: A developmental study. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 42, 7383.Google Scholar
Goswami U. 1991. Learning about spelling sequences: The role of onsets and rimes in analogies in reading. Child Development, 62, 11101123.Google Scholar
Goswami U. 2001. Early phonological development and the acquisition of literacy. In S. B. Neuman & D. K. Dickinson (Eds.), Handbook of early literacy research (pp. 111125). New York: Guilford Press.
Goswami U., Gombert J. E., & Barrera L. F. 1998. Children's orthographic representations and linguistic transparency: Nonsense word reading in English, French, and Spanish. Applied Psycholinguistics, 19, 1952.Google Scholar
Goswami U., & Mead F. 1992. Onset and rime awareness and analogies in reading. Reading Research Quarterly, 27, 153162.Google Scholar
Gyoyook Injuck Jawonboo [Korean Ministry of Education and Human Resources Development]. 2004. Yoochiwon hynwhang 2004 [Current status of kindergartens in 2004].

Retrieved October 14, 2004, from http://www.moe.go.kr/bbs/board.php?action=view&tn=moebbs&db=bbs1_1&id=94&cat menu=m05_02_03&SearchCode=all&search=

.Google Scholar
Hangook Yooah Gyoyook Yeongoowon [Korean Children Education Center]. (n.d.-a). Hangul chingu [Hangul friend]. Seoul, Korea: OOnggi Gyoyook.
Hangook Yooah Gyoyook Yeongoowon [Korean children education center]. 2003. Hangul jaragi [Hangul growth]. Seoul, Korea: Duckahm Publishing.
Hangook Yooah Gyoyook Yeongoowon [Korean Children Education Center]. (n.d.-b). Little Hangul wahng [Little Hangul King]. Seoul, Korea: Eduwang.
Hangook Yooah Gyoyook Yeongoowon [Korean Children Education Center]. (n.d.-c). Yeollin Hangul [Open Hangul]. Seoul, Korea: Yeollin gyoyook.
Hangooke Segewha choojineul weehan giban goochook saup keoyolgwa bogose [Report on the globalization of the Korean language]. 2005. Retrieved March 1, 2005, from http://letslearnkorean.com/downloads/mike-file1.pd.
Hangul gyoyook bangbupron (Methods of Hangul education). (2002, August 12). The Hangyeorae Times.
Hoien T., Lundberg I., Stanovich K. E., & Bjaalid I.-K. 1995. Components of phonological awareness. Reading and Writing: An Interdisciplinary Journal, 7, 171188.Google Scholar
Im J.-R. 1991. Kookeo kicho ewhiyeongooe tehan yeongoo [Study on the Korean basic vocabulary]. Kooke Kyoyook Yeongoo, 23, 8745.Google Scholar
Kang H., & Simpson G. B. 1996. Development of semantic and phonological priming in a shallow orthography. Developmental Psychology, 32, 860866.Google Scholar
Kessler B., & Treiman R. 1997. Syllable structure and the distribution of phonemes in English syllables. Journal of Memory and Language, 37, 295311.Google Scholar
Kim C.-W. 1997. The structure of phonological units in Han'gul. In Y. K. Kim-Reynaud (Ed.), The Korean alphabet (pp. 145160). Honolulu, HI: University of Hawaii Press.
Kim J.-H. 2001. Unowa moonwha [Language and Culture]. Seoul, Korea: Youkrack Publishing.
Korean Language. (2006, June 19). In Wikipedia, the free encyclopedia. Retrieved June 19, 2006, from http://en.wikipedia.org/wiki/Korean_language#Consonant.
Kyoyook Injuck Jawonboo [The Ministry of Education and Human Resources Development]. 2002. Kookeoh illggi, 1-1 [The Korean language: Reading, first semester of grade one]. Seoul, Korea: Daehan Kyokwaseo Jushikhweosa [Korean Textbooks Limited].
Lee K.-J., & Hahn M.-S. 1996. Kwango eneuy umwonloncek beunsek [Phonological analysis of advertisement language]. Kwanggo Hakbo, 1, 6172.Google Scholar
Lee Y.-S., Kwon K.-A., & Jung I.-S. 1980. Hankook Ahdonge ahwi baldalyeaongoo (I) [Korean children's vocabulary development]. Hankook Kyoyook Kaebalwon Yeongoobogo, 17, 75130.Google Scholar
Liberman I. Y., Shankweiler D., Fisher F., & Carter B. 1974. Explicit phoneme and syllable segmentation in the young child. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 18, 201212.Google Scholar
Lundberg I., Olofsson A., & Wall S. 1980. Reading and spelling skills in the first school years predicted from phonemic awareness skills in kindergarten. Scandinavian Journal of Psychology, 21, 159173.Google Scholar
McBride-Chang C. 1995. What is phonological awareness? Journal of Educational Psychology, 87, 179192.Google Scholar
Metsala J. L., & Walley A. C. 1998. Spoken vocabulary growth and the segmental restructuring of lexical representations: Precursors to phonemic awareness and early reading ability. In J. L. Metsala & L. C. Ehri (Eds.), Word recognition in beginning literacy (pp. 89120). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
Muller K., & Brady S. 2001. Correlates of early reading performance in a transparent orthography. Reading and Writing: An Interdisciplinary Journal, 14, 757799.Google Scholar
Na J., & Moon M. 2004. Early childhood education and care policies in the Republic of Korea (OECD Thematic Review of Early Childhood Education and Care Policy: Background Report). Retrieved October 14, 2004, from http://www.oecd.org/dataoecd/25/57/27856763.pd.
Oney B., & Durgonoglu A. Y. 1997. Beginning to read in Turkish: A phonologically transparent orthography. Applied Psycholinguistics, 18, 115.Google Scholar
Organisation for Economic Co-operation and Development. 2004. Early childhood education and care policy in the republic of Korea. OECD country note. Retrieved October 14, 2004 from http://www.oecd.org/dataoecd/42/43/33689774.pd.
Sampson G. 1985. Writing systems. Stanford, CA: Stanford University.
Share D. L., & Blum P. 2005. Syllable splitting in literate and preliterate Hebrew speakers: Onsets and rimes or bodies and codas? Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 92, 182202.Google Scholar
Sohn H.-M. 1999. The Korean language. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
Stahl S. A., & Murray B. A. 1994. Defining PA and its relationship to early reading. Journal of Educational Psychology, 86, 221234.Google Scholar
Storkel H. 2004. Do children acquire dense neighborhoods? An investigation of similarity neighborhoods in lexical acquisition. Applied Psycholinguistics, 25, 201221.Google Scholar
Taylor I. 1980. The Korean writing system: An alphabet? a syllabary? a logography? In P. A. Kolers, M. E. Wrolstad, & H. Bouma (Eds.), Processing of visible language (pp. 6782). New York: Plenum Press.
Treiman R. 1985. Onsets and rimes as units of spoken syllables: Evidence from children. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 39, 161181.Google Scholar
Treiman R. 1992. The role of intrasyllabic units in leaning to read and spell. In P. B. Gough, L. C. Ehri, & R. Treiman (Eds.), Reading acquisition (pp. 65106). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
Treiman R., Mullennix J., Bijeljac-Babic R., & Richmond-Welty E. D. 1995. The special role of rimes in the description, use and acquisition of English orthography. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 124, 107136.Google Scholar
Treiman R., Tincoff R., & Richmond-Welty D. 1996. Letter names help children to connect print and speech. Developmental Psychology, 32, 505514.Google Scholar
Treiman R., & Zukowski A. 1991. Level of PA. In S. A. Brady & D. P. Shankweiler (Eds.), Phonological processes in literacy (pp. 6784). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
Yi K.-O. 1998. The internal structure of the syllable in Korean: Onset–rime or body-coda? Korean Journal of Experimental and Cognitive Psychology, 10, 6783.Google Scholar
Yoon H. K., Bolger D. J., Kwon O.-S., & Perfetti C. A. 2002. Subysllabic units in reading. In L. Verhoeven, C. Elbro, & P. Reitsma (Eds.), Precursors of functional literacy (Vol. 11, pp. 139163). Amsterdam: Benjamins.
Yoon Y.-B., & Derwing B. (1995, August). Syllable saliency in the perception of Korean words. Paper presented at the XIIIth International Congress of Phonetic Sciences, Stockholm, Sweden.
Yoon Y.-B., & Derwing B. 2001. A language without a rhyme: Syllable structure experiments in Korean. Canadian Journal of Linguistics, 46, 187237.Google Scholar
Ziegler J. C., & Goswami U. 2005. Reading acquisition, developmental dyslexia, and skilled reading across languages: A psycholinguistic grain size theory. Psychological Bulletin, 131, 329.Google Scholar