General intellectual ability does not explain the general deficit in schizophrenia

https://doi.org/10.1016/j.schres.2013.04.016Get rights and content

Abstract

Patients with schizophrenia demonstrate a generalized deficit across multiple cognitive domains. However, it is unknown whether this deficit is largely due to lower intelligence, or if there is an impact of schizophrenia which cannot be accounted for by measures of general intellectual ability (GIA). We created four IQ-matched strata of equal width between 89 healthy volunteers (HC) and 77 patients with schizophrenia (SZ) who had very similar IQ and reading scores within each stratum, then compared each stratum's performance on the MATRICS Consensus Cognitive Battery (MCCB). We hypothesized that any patient impairment on the MCCB after matching on IQ would be evidence that GIA does not fully explain the general deficit seen in schizophrenia. We found that patients showed evidence of greater neuropsychological impairment than what would be expected based solely on their IQ and reading ability scores. Further, this deficit was stronger in some cognitive domains than others, namely, processing speed and social cognition. These results suggest the presence of a distinction between GIA and generalized neuropsychological impairment that was consistent in magnitude across all patients, regardless of IQ.

Introduction

Meta-analyses provide overwhelming evidence that patients with schizophrenia demonstrate marked deficits across cognitive domains (Heinrichs and Zakzanis, 1998, Fioravanti et al., 2005, Dickinson et al., 2007, Reichenberg and Harvey, 2007, Reichenberg, 2010). While there is variability in the extent of impairment, evidence suggests that impairment is generalized across the cognitive operations assessed by widely used clinical neuropsychological measures. Further, it appears that the extent of impairment across these domains (i.e., attention, processing speed, working memory, etc.), is highly intercorrelated. Dickinson et al. (2008) used structural equation modeling to demonstrate that 64% of the between-group variance in neuropsychological performance between healthy controls and individuals with schizophrenia is shared on a common general deficit factor, with more specific deficits accounting for very little additional between-group variance (Dickinson et al., 2004, Dickinson et al., 2008).

With evidence of a generalized deficit across cognitive domains, the question arises whether the “general deficit” might simply be a reflection of a reduction in general intellectual ability (GIA), i.e., intelligence. Indeed, IQ measures are typically highly correlated with neuropsychological performance. For example, in a sample of 117 individuals with schizophrenia (SZ), WASI-estimated IQ scores correlated with the composite score from the MATRICS Consensus Cognitive Battery (MCCB, Kern et al., 2008, Nuechterlein et al., 2008), r = .733, p < .001 with a very similar correlation observed in a sample of 77 healthy controls (HCs), r = .695, p < .001 (August et al., 2012). These substantial correlations are noteworthy because the MCCB was deliberately composed of measures particularly impaired in schizophrenia and/or particularly important for functional outcome. Thus, one would expect to see a schizophrenia deficit “signal” in MCCB performance that extends beyond GIA. We speculate that across the WASI and MCCB there are two “pools” of variance: 1) a pool of variance associated with GIA reflected in the high correlation of the two measures, and 2) a pool of variance associated with the impact of schizophrenia on more discrete aspects of cognitive function that are captured on the MCCB which cannot be accounted for by GIA.

We took two approaches to this issue. First, we compared the MCCB performance of healthy volunteers and patients with schizophrenia who had very similar WASI IQ scores. If IQ accounts for neuropsychological performance across groups, the IQ-matched groups should show similar levels of performance on the MCCB. Alternatively, any patient impairment on the MCCB, after matching on IQ, would be evidence that the “general deficit” and GIA are not synonymous. In addition, we performed the same matched group approach using measures of single word reading which are thought to index “premorbid” ability (Spreen and Strauss, 1998, Lezak et al., 2004). With both the WASI and reading measures, this approach addresses the question of whether patients are more impaired than they “should” be for their level of reading and IQ performance, and allows for a quantitative estimate of how far patients deviate from the level that would be expected had they not become ill. We examined these questions by creating groups that ranged from low to high levels of GIA to provide additional information about whether patients who have higher levels of cognitive ability are spared the neuropsychological impairments that have most frequently been documented in samples with average–low average levels of GIA. Second, we used an ANCOVA approach that provides further information on whether patients show greater impairments in some domains than others after controlling for the role of GIA.

Section snippets

Participants

Participants in the full sample included 143 individuals with a DSM-IV diagnosis of schizophrenia or schizoaffective disorder (SZ) as confirmed by the Structured Clinical Interview for DSM-IV (SCID; First et al., 2002). SZs were recruited from the Maryland Psychiatric Research Center and other community clinics. SZs were found to be clinically stable by their clinicians and had been receiving stable psychotropic medication with no changes to type or dosage for four weeks prior to testing.

Descriptive statistics

Table 1 represents the demographic information of the restricted sample used in the analysis. In the restricted sample, HCs and SZs were closely matched on all variables except that HCs had, on average, approximately 1.3 more years of education than did SZs, t (164) = 4.42, p < .001. However, in this selected sample of SZs, average paternal education was about 1.5 years higher than in fathers of HCs, t (164) =  2.86, p < .01. The SZ group also included more males than the HC (82% versus 64%, χ2 = 9.53, p < 

Discussion

These results offer a somewhat different perspective on the generalized nature of neuropsychological impairment in schizophrenia. Most importantly, our results demonstrate a clear distinction between GIA (as reflected in IQ) and generalized neuropsychological impairment. Patients and controls at similar IQ levels demonstrate very different MCCB performance (generalized neuropsychological impairment). Co-varying for IQ and reading ability did not eliminate between-group differences across the

Role of funding source

This study is funded by the National Institutes of Health US (NIH). The NIMH did not play a role in the design of the study, data collection, data analysis, or manuscript preparation.

Contributors

Bradley E. Gray gathered the study data, did basic analyses, and wrote the initial and final draft of the paper. Robert P. McMahon completed the advanced statistical analysis. James M. Gold conducted the initial literature search, wrote an initial draft of the introduction and discussion, and edited the paper. All authors approved the final manuscript.

Conflict of interest

Author Robert P. McMahon has been a statistical consultant for Amgen, Inc. within the past 3 years.

Acknowledgments

This work was supported by NIMH MH065034 and NIMH RO1 MH080066. We offer thanks to Samuel Kaiser, Jacqueline Kiwanuka, Leeka Hubzin, Sharon August, Benjamin Robinson, and Alexander Harvey for their work in the coordination and conduct of this study.

References (20)

  • S.M. August et al.

    The MATRICS Consensus Cognitive Battery (MCCB): clinical and cognitive correlates

    Schizophr. Res.

    (2012)
  • D. Dickinson et al.

    General and specific cognitive deficits in schizophrenia

    Biol. Psychiatry

    (2004)
  • D. Dickinson et al.

    General and specific cognitive deficits in schizophrenia: Goliath defeats David?

    Biol. Psychiatry

    (2008)
  • Y. Benjamini et al.

    Controlling the false discovery rate: a new and powerful approach to multiple significance testing

    J. R. Stat. Soc., Ser. B

    (1995)
  • D. Dickinson et al.

    Overlooking the obvious: a meta-analytic comparison of digit symbol coding tasks and other cognitive measures in schizophrenia

    Arch. Gen. Psychiatry

    (2007)
  • M. Fioravanti et al.

    A meta-analysis of cognitive deficits in adults with a diagnosis of schizophrenia

    Neuropsychol. Rev.

    (2005)
  • M.B. First et al.

    Structured Clinical Interview for DSM–IV–TR Axis I Disorders, Research Version, Patient Edition With Psychotic Screen

    (2002)
  • R. Heinrichs et al.

    Neurocognitive deficit in schizophrenia: a quantitative review of the evidence

    Neuropsychology

    (1998)
  • R.S. Kern

    The MATRICS Consensus Cognitive Battery, part 2: co-norming and standardization

    Am. J. Psychiatry

    (2008)
  • M.D. Lezak et al.

    Neuropsychological Assessment

    (2004)
There are more references available in the full text version of this article.

Cited by (13)

  • Associations between facial affect recognition and neurocognition in subjects at ultra-high risk for psychosis: A case-control study

    2020, Psychiatry Research
    Citation Excerpt :

    Fisher's r-to-z transformation (Cohen, 1988) was applied to compare partial correlation coefficients between groups (Dyck et al., 2006). Of note, we included both an intelligence estimate and a neurocognitive composite in these analyses; these constructs are related but non-redundant given that the amount of variance in global neurocognitive test performance explained by intelligence is significant but not complete (Gray et al., 2013; Mohn et al., 2014). Mann-Whitney test compared group performances across DFAR Task specific affective conditions.

  • Neurocognitive normality in schizophrenia revisited

    2015, Schizophrenia Research: Cognition
  • Brain structure characteristics in intellectually superior schizophrenia

    2015, Psychiatry Research - Neuroimaging
    Citation Excerpt :

    Individuals with SZ who score within the normal range on neuropsychological tests have reductions of one kind or another, such as deficits in attention and executive functioning in spite of preserved IQ (Weickert et al., 2000), or lower processing speed and memory scores compared with IQ-matched HC participants (Wilk et al., 2005). Neurocognitive decrements are present in practically all SZ cases (Keefe et al., 2005), even in individuals with SZ and superior intellectual abilities (MacCabe et al., 2012; Gray et al., 2013). In a recent study (Vaskinn et al., 2014), we found that individuals diagnosed with SZ with IQ scores>120 had the same magnitude of neurocognitive decrements as those with normal or low intelligence when compared with IQ-matched HC individuals.

  • The adenosine neuromodulation system in schizophrenia

    2014, International Review of Neurobiology
    Citation Excerpt :

    Thus, there seems to be an ability of A1R to control startling and PPI, which might be therapeutically exploited by increasing the extracellular levels of adenosine through the inhibition of adenosine kinase, whereas the role of A2AR is still far from clear. Schizophrenics demonstrate incapacitating cognitive dysfunction (Elvevag & Goldberg, 2000) not related with intellectual disability (Gray, Mcmahon, & Gold, 2013). The general trend of these deficits are more marked in later life and are an important treatment target, because such deficits are the best predictor of a patient's level of interpersonal skills, occupational functioning, and self care (Bowie & Harvey, 2005).

  • A model of memory impairment in schizophrenia: Cognitive and clinical factors associated with memory efficiency and memory errors

    2013, Schizophrenia Research
    Citation Excerpt :

    Patients with schizophrenia demonstrate cognitive dysfunction that might be as incapacitating as their positive and negative symptoms (Elvevag and Golbdberg, 2000) and that cannot be explained merely by general intellectual disability (Gray et al., 2013).

View all citing articles on Scopus
View full text