Relations of academic and general self-esteem to school achievement

https://doi.org/10.1016/j.paid.2008.06.017Get rights and content

Abstract

The study demonstrates on a nationally representative sample of Estonian students and university applicants (N = 4572) that although self-reported academic self-esteem is a strong and accurate predictor of school achievement, additionally rather low, not high, general self-esteem is a significant predictor of superior school performance when academic self-esteem and multicollinearity is controlled for. Two compensatory mechanisms—defensive pessimism and self-protective enhancement—may explain the paradox of low self-esteem: academically successful students have a more critical view of themselves and students with more modest academic abilities compensate for their academic under-achievement by elevating their general self-esteem. Children start to use self-protective enhancement but from age 12 to 14 they also start using defensive pessimism to protect themselves from the consequences of failure.

Introduction

Despite popular beliefs that high self-esteem facilitates academic achievement, only a modest correlation was discovered between general self-esteem and school performance (Byrne, 1984, Byrne and Shavelson, 1986, Hansford and Hattie, 1982, Marsh and Yeung, 1998, Mintz and Muller, 1977). A recent meta-analysis estimated that the effect of the favourable influence of positive self-beliefs on academic achievement is modest and the overall estimated relation is about .08 (Valentine, Dubois, & Cooper, 2004). Also longitudinal studies do not point to any significant role for general self-esteem in the advancing of academic performance (Bachman and O’Malley, 1977, Byrne, 1986, Helmke and van Aken, 1995, Maruyama et al., 1981, Pottebaum et al., 1986, Skaalvik and Hagtveg, 1990). Educational practices attempting to boost children’s self-worth have demonstrated only limited and temporal effects unless they targeted specific self-concept domains (O’Mara et al., 2006). Based on these observations, Baumeister and his colleagues (2003) concluded that generalized evaluation of self-worth has no significant impact on the subsequent academic achievement (see Marsh & Craven, 2006, for a different opinion).

One possible explanation to the absence of a strong association between general self-esteem and academic performance is the specificity matching principle: in order to expect a strong or moderate correlation the specificity of predictors and criteria should be matched (Swann, Chang-Schneider, & McClarty, 2007). Because general self-esteem is unspecific and school performance rather narrowly defined, it may seem unlikely that school grades can be predicted from what people think or at least report about their general self-worth.

Not disputing the specificity matching principle, it is relevant to recognize that self-concept is a multidimensional construct (cf. Shavelson, Hubner, & Stanton, 1976) in which general and academic self-esteem are two distinguishable components with two dissimilar functions. While general self-esteem appears to be heavily affective in nature and tends to be associated with overall psychological well-being, specific self-esteem—that is self-evaluations in narrowly defined domains like school performance—appears to have a more cognitive component and tends to be more strongly associated with behaviour or behavioural outcomes (Baumeister et al., 2003, Rosenberg et al., 1995). This means, in particular, that general self-esteem is only partly based on the student’s academic self-esteem defined as evaluation of their school performance while self-assessment of their academic abilities is sufficiently accurate to reflect their achievement at school. Indeed, a meta-analysis has shown that specific academic self-concepts provide much better prediction of academic achievements than global self-esteem (Hansford & Hattie, 1982).

The main goal of this study is to demonstrate a multidimensional character of self-concept by showing reciprocation between general and academic self-esteem in their joint prediction of school achievement.

Section snippets

Participants

Three samples with a total of 4572 Estonian students and university applicants participated in this study. The number of participants with their mean ages across the grades is reported in Table 1.

Results

The mean level of GPA regularly decreased through all the grades starting from the highest value of 4.25 in Grade 2 and reaching its lowest value of 3.80 in Grade 12 (Table 1). As expected, the university applicants had statistically significantly higher GPA than secondary school leavers (d = .69, p < .001). Across all samples, girls had higher GPA than boys [m = 4.17 vs. 3.82, respectively; t(4600) = 19.66, p < .001] and this advantage of girls remained in each study group. The students’ academic

Discussion

The main finding of the present study is a mixture of expected and surprising results: although academic self-esteem systematically and accurately predicts school achievement, students’ opinions about their general self-worth also have some associations with academic accomplishments: After elementary school, students with lower general self-esteem are more likely to be academically successful when their self-rated academic self-esteem is taken into account.

This is certainly not the first

Acknowledgements

This project was supported by the Estonian Science Foundation (Grant 7020) and the Estonian Ministry of Science and Education (Grant SF0180029s08). We thank Kaia Laidra and Liisa Raudsepp for their assistance with collecting the data.

References (44)

  • J.G. Bachman et al.

    Self-esteem in young men: A longitudinal analysis of the impact of educational and occupational attainment

    Journal of Personality and Social Psychology

    (1977)
  • R.F. Baumeister et al.

    Does high self-esteem cause better performance, interpersonal success, happiness, or healthier lifestyles?

    Psychological Science in the Public Interest

    (2003)
  • M. Bong et al.

    Comparison between self-concept and self-efficacy in academic motivation research

    Educational Psychologist

    (1999)
  • B.M. Byrne

    The general academic self-concept nomological network: A review of construct-validation research

    Review of Educational Research

    (1984)
  • B.M. Byrne

    Self-concept/academic achievement relations: An investigation of dimensionality, stability, and causality

    Canadian Journal of Behavioural Science

    (1986)
  • B.M. Byrne et al.

    On the structure of the adolescent self-concept

    Journal of Educational Psychology

    (1986)
  • J. Crocker et al.

    Contingencies of self-worth

    Psychological Review

    (2001)
  • B. Gray-Little et al.

    Factors influencing racial comparisons of self-esteem: A quantitative review

    Psychological Bulletin

    (2000)
  • B.C. Hansford et al.

    The relationship between self and achievement/performance measures

    Review of Educational Research

    (1982)
  • J. Hattie

    Self-concept

    (1992)
  • A. Helmke et al.

    The causal ordering of academic achievement and self-concept of ability during elementary school: A longitudinal study

    Journal of Educational Psychology

    (1995)
  • K.C. Kling et al.

    Gender differences in self-esteem: A meta-analysis

    Psychological Bulletin

    (1999)
  • K. Laidra et al.

    Personality and intelligence as predictors of academic achievement: A cross-sectional study from elementary to secondary school

    Personality and Individual Differences

    (2007)
  • H.W. Marsh

    Content specificity of relations between academic-achievement and academic self-concept

    Journal of Educational Psychology

    (1992)
  • H.W. Marsh et al.

    Reciprocal effects of self-concepts and performance from a multidimensional perspective

    Perspectives on Psychological Science

    (2006)
  • H.W. Marsh et al.

    Reciprocal effects between academic self-concept, self-esteem, achievement, and attainment over seven adolescent years: Unidimensional and multidimensional perspectives of self-concept

    Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin

    (2008)
  • H.W. Marsh et al.

    To-down, bottom-up, and horizontal models: The direction of causality in multidimensional, hierarchical self-concept models

    Journal of Personality and Social Psychology

    (1998)
  • G. Maruyama et al.

    Self-esteem and educational achievement: Independent constructs or a common cause

    Journal of Personality and Social Psychology

    (1981)
  • R. Mintz et al.

    Academic achievement as a function of specific and global measures of self-concept

    Journal of Psychology

    (1977)
  • C.C. Morf et al.

    Unraveling the paradoxes of narcissism: A dynamic self-regulatory processing model

    Psychological Inquiry

    (2001)
  • Cited by (113)

    • The impact of working with core qualities on primary school pupils’ self-concept

      2021, International Journal of Educational Research Open
      Citation Excerpt :

      In addition, prior research has shown that school performance and school self-concept are strongly interrelated (Marsh & O'Mara, 2008). Pullmann and Allik (2008) show that school self-concept influences school performance, as compared with a high overall self-concept, which is not usually a reliable predictor of school performance. Important sources that influence pupils’ self-concept, according to the multidimensional perspective, other than their own perception are feedback from others and cultural values (Weiten et al., 2014).

    View all citing articles on Scopus
    View full text