Elsevier

NeuroImage

Volume 200, 15 October 2019, Pages 142-158
NeuroImage

Beyond consensus: Embracing heterogeneity in curated neuroimaging meta-analysis

https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2019.06.037Get rights and content

Abstract

Coordinate-based meta-analysis can provide important insights into mind-brain relationships. A popular approach for curated small-scale meta-analysis is activation likelihood estimation (ALE), which identifies brain regions consistently activated across a selected set of experiments, such as within a functional domain or mental disorder. ALE can also be utilized in meta-analytic co-activation modeling (MACM) to identify brain regions consistently co-activated with a seed region. Therefore, ALE aims to find consensus across experiments, treating heterogeneity across experiments as noise. However, heterogeneity within an ALE analysis of a functional domain might indicate the presence of functional sub-domains. Similarly, heterogeneity within a MACM analysis might indicate the involvement of a seed region in multiple co-activation patterns that are dependent on task contexts. Here, we demonstrate the use of the author-topic model to automatically determine if heterogeneities within ALE-type meta-analyses can be robustly explained by a small number of latent patterns. In the first application, the author-topic modeling of experiments involving self-generated thought (N = 179) revealed cognitive components fractionating the default network. In the second application, the author-topic model revealed that the left inferior frontal junction (IFJ) participated in multiple task-dependent co-activation patterns (N = 323). Furthermore, the author-topic model estimates compared favorably with spatial independent component analysis in both simulation and real data. Overall, the results suggest that the author-topic model is a flexible tool for exploring heterogeneity in ALE-type meta-analyses that might arise from functional sub-domains, mental disorder subtypes or task-dependent co-activation patterns. Code for this study is publicly available (https://github.com/ThomasYeoLab/CBIG/tree/master/stable_projects/meta-analysis/Ngo2019_AuthorTopic).

Introduction

Brain imaging experiments are often underpowered (Carp, 2012; Poline et al., 2012; Button et al., 2013). Coordinate-based meta-analysis provides an important framework for analyzing underpowered studies across different experimental conditions and analysis piplines to reveal reliable trends (Wager et al., 2003; Fox et al., 2014; Poldrack and Yarkoni, 2016). Large-scale coordinate-based meta-analyses synthesize thousands of experiments across diverse experimental designs to discover broad and general principles of brain organization and disorder (Laird et al., 2011; Poldrack et al., 2012; Crossley et al., 2014). By contrast, the vast majority of meta-analyses involve smaller number of experiments that are expertly chosen (curated) to generate consensus on specific functional domains (e.g., Binder et al., 2009), brain regions (e.g., Shackman et al., 2011) or disorders (e.g., Cortese et al., 2012).

A popular approach for smaller-scale meta-analyses is activation likelihood estimation or ALE (Turkeltaub et al., 2002; Laird et al., 2005; Eickhoff et al., 2009, 2012; Turkeltaub et al., 2012). ALE identifies brain regions consistently activated across neuroimaging experiments within a functional domain (Costafreda et al., 2008; Spaniol et al., 2009; Beissner et al., 2013) or within a disorder (e.g., Fitzgerald et al., 2008; Minzenberg et al., 2009; Di Martino et al., 2009). Thus, ALE treats heterogeneities across studies as noise. Consequently, ALE analysis might miss out on genuine biological heterogeneity indicative of functional sub-domains or disorder subtypes.

For example, Fig. 1 (middle panel) illustrates activation foci from experiments across multiple tasks associated with a hypothetical functional domain. These foci are generated by two latent sub-domains activating distinct, but overlapping, brain regions. Without prior knowledge of the two sub-domains from theory or previous empirical work, ALE will converge on regions commonly activated across both sub-domains (Fig. 1 left panel). To get around this issue, meta-analytic studies can sub-divide experiments into hypothetical functional sub-domains before applying ALE. For example, a recent meta-analysis divided working memory experiments into verbal versus non-verbal tasks, as well as tasks involving object identity versus object locations (Rottschy et al., 2012). However, manually subdividing experiments requires prior knowledge of the sub-domains and may reinforce biases towards existing concepts. By contrast, in this study, we explored whether a previously published data-driven approach (author-topic model; Yeo et al., 2015) can help uncover heterogeneities1 within ALE-type meta-analyses in a bottom-up, data-driven fashion (Fig. 1 right panel).

A good example in which ALE might miss out on functional sub-domains is the default network and self-generated thought (Smallwood, 2013; Andrews-Hanna et al., 2014). Self-generated thought involves associative and constructive processes that take place within an individual, and depends upon an internal representation to reconstruct or imagine a situation, understand a stimulus, or generate an answer to a question. The term “self-generated thought” serves to contrast with thoughts where the primary referent is based on immediate perceptual input. By virtue of being largely stimulus independent or task unrelated, self-generated thought has been linked with the functions of the default network (Buckner et al., 2008; Andrews-Hanna et al., 2014). Previous ALE meta-analyses have implicated the default network in many tasks involving self-generated thought, including theory of mind, narrative fiction, autobiographical memory and moral cognition (Spreng et al., 2009; Binder et al., 2009; Mar, 2011; Sevinc and Spreng, 2014).

However, many studies have suggested that the default network might be fractionated into sub-systems. For example, Andrews-Hanna and colleagues have proposed a dorsomedial prefrontal subsystem preferentially specialized for social cognition and narrative processing (Andrews-Hanna et al., 2014; Spreng and Andrews-Hanna, 2015) and a medial temporal lobe sub-system preferentially specialized for mnemonic constructive processes (Andrews-Hanna et al., 2014; Christoff et al., 2016). Both sub-systems might spatially overlap or inter-digitate across multiple brain regions (Andrews-Hanna et al., 2014; Braga and Buckner, 2017), which would be challenging to ALE without assuming prior knowledge of the sub-systems (Fig. 1). Furthermore, specific default network fractionation details differed across studies (Laird et al., 2009a; Andrews-Hanna et al., 2010b; Mayer et al., 2010; Humphreys et al., 2015; Kernbach et al., 2018), so application of the author-topic model might potentially clarify sub-systems subserving self-generated thought.

Another common application of ALE is meta-analytic connectivity modeling (MACM), which identifies brain regions that consistently co-activate with a particular seed region (Toro et al., 2008; Koski and Paus, 2000; Robinson et al., 2010; Eickhoff et al., 2010). The assumption is that the seed region exhibits a single co-activation pattern regardless of the actual task activating the seed region (Robinson et al., 2010). However, studies have shown the existence of multiple hub regions in the brain (e.g., dorsal anterior insula, dorsal anterior cingulate cortex) that are activated across many different tasks and might adapt their connectivity pattern depending on task context (Cole et al., 2013; Uddin, 2015; Bertolero et al., 2017). Thus, a seed region might be involved in multiple task-dependent co-activation patterns (McIntosh, 2000).

A good example in which MACM might miss out on multiple co-activation patterns is the left inferior frontal junction (IFJ; Muhle-Karbe et al., 2015). The IFJ has been implicated in many cognitive processes (Brass et al., 2005; Chikazoe et al., 2009a, Chikazoe et al., 2009b; Asplund et al., 2010) and is a key node of the multiple-demand system (Duncan, 2010; Fedorenko et al., 2013). IFJ might also coordinate information among modules by adapting its connectivity patterns across different resting and task states (Cole et al., 2013; Bertolero et al., 2018). Therefore, one might expect the IFJ region to exhibit multiple co-activation patterns that are dependent on task contexts. Since ALE cannot capture heterogeneity across experiments, MACM might be insensitive to such task-dependent co-activation patterns. On the other hand, application of the author-topic model to the IFJ region might yield multiple meaningful co-activation patterns.

In this work, we propose the use of the author-topic model to automatically make sense of heterogeneity within ALE-type meta-analyses. We have previously utilized the author-topic model (Fig. 2; Yeo et al., 2015; Bertolero et al., 2015) to encode the intuitive notion that a behavioral task recruits multiple cognitive components, which are in turn supported by overlapping brain regions (Poldrack, 2006; Leech et al., 2012; Barrett and Satpute, 2013). While our previous work focused on large-scale meta-analysis across many functional domains (Yeo et al., 2015; Bertolero et al., 2015), the current study focuses on heterogeneity within a functional domain (self-generated thought) or co-activation heterogeneity of a seed region (left IFJ). These applications of the author-topic model are made possible by the development of a novel inference algorithm for the author-topic model (Ngo et al., 2016) that is sufficiently robust for smaller-scale meta-analyses.

Our choice of self-generated thought is motivated by previous work suggesting the possibility of fractionating self-generated thought into functional sub-domains (Section 1.1). Similarly, our choice of left IFJ is motivated by previous work suggesting that IFJ might adaptively modify its connectivity patterns across task contexts (Section 1.2). There are of course other functional domains (e.g., executive function) that might be fractionated and other hub regions (e.g., dorsal anterior insula) that might exhibit task-dependent co-activation patterns. Therefore, we have made our code publicly available for researchers to explore the heterogeneity of their preferred functional domain, hub region or mental disorder.

Section snippets

Overview

In Section 2.2, we reviewed the author-topic model and how it could be applied to coordinate-based meta-analysis (Yeo et al., 2015). Section 2.3 discussed simulations and comparisons with spatial independent component analysis. Finally, the model was utilized in two different applications. In the first application (Section 2.4), we applied the author-topic model to discover cognitive components subserving self-generated thought. In the second application (Section 2.5), we estimated the

Overview

In Section 3.2, we show simulation results suggesting that the author-topic model compares favorably with ICA in the goal of discovering latent patterns in coordinate-based meta-analysis. We then explored the cognitive components of self-generated thought (Section 3.3) and the co-activation patterns of the IFJ (Section 3.4). Finally, Section 3.5 discusses a few control analyses.

Simulations

Fig. 5 shows the results of one representative simulation (see Section 2.3 for details). Fig. 5A shows the groundtruth

Discussion

The author-topic model encodes the intuitive notion that behavioral tasks recruited multiple cognitive components, supported by multiple brain regions (Mesulam, 1990; Poldrack, 2006; Barrett and Satpute, 2013). We have previously utilized the author-topic model for large-scale meta-analysis across functional domains (Yeo et al., 2015; Bertolero et al., 2015). By exploiting a recently developed CVB algorithm for the author-topic model (Ngo et al., 2016), we show that the model can also be

Conclusion

Heterogeneities across neuroimaging experiments are often treated as noise in coordinate-based meta-analyses. Here we demonstrate that the author-topic model can be utilized to determine if the heterogeneities can be explained by a small number of latent patterns. In the first application, the author-topic model revealed two overlapping cognitive components subserving self-generated thought. In the second application, the author-topic revealed the participation of the left IFJ in three

Acknowledgements

This work was supported by Singapore MOE Tier 2 (MOE2014-T2-2-016), NUS Strategic Research (DPRT/944/09/14), NUS SOM Aspiration Fund (R185000271720), Singapore NMRC (CBRG/0088/2015), NUS YIA and the Singapore National Research Foundation Fellowship (Class of 2017). Simon Eickhoff is supported by the National Institute of Mental Health (R01-MH074457), the Helmholtz Portfolio Theme “Supercomputing and Modeling for the Human Brain” and the European Union’s Horizon 2020 Research and Innovation

References (160)

  • A. Abraham et al.

    Machine learning for neuroimaging with scikit-learn

    Front. Neuroinf.

    (2014)
  • J.R. Andrews-Hanna et al.

    Evidence for the default networks role in spontaneous cognition

    J. Neurophysiol.

    (2010)
  • J.R. Andrews-Hanna et al.

    Functional-anatomic fractionation of the brains default network

    Neuron

    (2010)
  • J.R. Andrews-Hanna et al.

    The default network and self-generated thought: component processes, dynamic control, and clinical relevance

    Ann. N. Y. Acad. Sci.

    (2014)
  • J.R. Andrews-Hanna

    The brain's default network and its adaptive role in internal mentation

    Neuroscientist

    (2012)
  • C.L. Asplund et al.

    A central role for the lateral prefrontal cortex in goal-directed and stimulus-driven attention

    Nat. Neurosci.

    (2010)
  • C.M. Atance et al.

    Episodic future thinking

    Trends Cognit. Sci.

    (2001)
  • B. Baird et al.

    Back to the future: autobiographical planning and the functionality of mind-wandering

    Conscious. Cognit.

    (2011)
  • S.C. Baker et al.

    The interaction between mood and cognitive function studied with PET

    Psychol. Med.

    (1997)
  • D. Baldauf et al.

    Neural mechanisms of object-based attention

    Science

    (2014)
  • D.M. Barch et al.

    Anterior cingulate cortex and response conflict: effects of response modality and processing domain

    Cerebr. Cortex

    (2001)
  • L.F. Barrett et al.

    Large-scale brain networks in affective and social neuroscience: towards an integrative functional architecture of the brain

    Curr. Opin. Neurobiol.

    (2013)
  • C.F. Beckmann et al.

    Probabilistic independent component analysis for functional magnetic resonance imaging

    IEEE Trans. Med. Imaging

    (2004)
  • F. Beissner et al.

    The autonomic brain: an activation likelihood estimation meta-analysis for central processing of autonomic function

    J. Neurosci.

    (2013)
  • K.F. Berman et al.

    Physiological activation of a cortical network during performance of the Wisconsin Card Sorting Test: a positron emission tomography study

    Neuropsychologia

    (1995)
  • M.A. Bertolero et al.

    The modular and integrative functional architecture of the human brain

    Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. Unit. States Am.

    (2015)
  • M.A. Bertolero et al.

    The diverse club

    Nat. Commun.

    (2017)
  • M.A. Bertolero et al.

    A mechanistic model of connector hubs, modularity, and cognition

    Nature Human Behaviour

    (2018)
  • J.R. Binder et al.

    Where is the semantic system? A critical review and meta-analysis of 120 functional neuroimaging studies

    Cerebr. Cortex

    (2009)
  • J.R. Binder et al.

    The neurobiology of semantic memory

    Trends Cognit. Sci.

    (2011)
  • R.M. Braga et al.

    Parallel interdigitated distributed networks within the individual estimated by intrinsic functional connectivity

    Neuron

    (2017)
  • M. Brass et al.

    The role of the inferior frontal junction area in cognitive control

    Trends Cognit. Sci.

    (2005)
  • M. Brass et al.

    The role of the frontal cortex in task preparation

    Cerebr. Cortex

    (2002)
  • T.S. Braver et al.

    Anterior cingulate cortex and response conflict: effects of frequency, inhibition and errors

    Cerebr. Cortex

    (2001)
  • A.D. Brown et al.

    Episodic and semantic components of autobiographical memories and imagined future events in post-traumatic stress disorder

    Memory

    (2014)
  • R.L. Buckner et al.

    The brains default network

    Ann. N. Y. Acad. Sci.

    (2008)
  • R.L. Buckner et al.

    Self-projection and the brain

    Trends Cognit. Sci.

    (2007)
  • R.L. Buckner et al.

    The organization of the human cerebellum estimated by intrinsic functional connectivity

    J. Neurophysiol.

    (2011)
  • N. Burgess et al.

    The human hippocampus and spatial and episodic memory

    Neuron

    (2002)
  • K.S. Button et al.

    Power failure: why small sample size undermines the reliability of neuroscience

    Nat. Rev. Neurosci.

    (2013)
  • P. Byrne et al.

    Remembering the past and imagining the future: a neural model of spatial memory and imagery

    Psychol. Rev.

    (2007)
  • D. Bzdok et al.

    Segregation of the human medial prefrontal cortex in social cognition

    Front. Hum. Neurosci.

    (2013)
  • V.D. Calhoun et al.

    A method for making group inferences from functional MRI data using independent component analysis

    Human Brain Mmapping

    (2001)
  • D.E. Callan et al.

    Neural correlates of the spacing effect in explicit verbal semantic encoding support the deficient-processing theory

    Hum. Brain Mapp.

    (2010)
  • F. Callard et al.

    What we talk about when we talk about the default mode network

    Front. Hum. Neurosci.

    (2014)
  • J. Carp

    The secret lives of experiments: methods reporting in the fMRI literature

    Neuroimage

    (2012)
  • J. Chikazoe et al.

    Functional dissociation in right inferior frontal cortex during performance of go/no-go task

    Cerebr. Cortex

    (2009)
  • J. Chikazoe et al.

    Preparation to inhibit a response complements response inhibition during performance of a stop-signal task

    J. Neurosci.

    (2009)
  • K. Christoff et al.

    Mind-wandering as spontaneous thought: a dynamic framework

    Nat. Rev. Neurosci.

    (2016)
  • M.W. Cole et al.

    Multi-task connectivity reveals flexible hubs for adaptive task control

    Nat. Neurosci.

    (2013)
  • S. Cortese et al.

    Toward systems neuroscience of ADHD: a meta-analysis of 55 fMRI studies

    Am. J. Psychiatry

    (2012)
  • S.G. Costafreda et al.

    Predictors of amygdala activation during the processing of emotional stimuli: a meta-analysis of 385 PET and fMRI studies

    Brain Res. Rev.

    (2008)
  • N.A. Crossley et al.

    The hubs of the human connectome are generally implicated in the anatomy of brain disorders

    Brain

    (2014)
  • J.F. Démonet et al.

    A PET study of cognitive strategies in normal subjects during language tasks: influence of phonetic ambiguity and sequence processing on phoneme monitoring

    Brain

    (1994)
  • J. Derrfuss et al.

    Involvement of the inferior frontal junction in cognitive control: meta-analyses of switching and Stroop studies

    Hum. Brain Mapp.

    (2005)
  • J. Derrfuss et al.

    Cognitive control in the posterior frontolateral cortex: evidence from common activations in task coordination, interference control, and working memory

    Neuroimage

    (2004)
  • R. Desai et al.

    FMRI of past tense processing: the effects of phonological complexity and task difficulty

    J. Cogn. Neurosci.

    (2006)
  • A. Di Martino et al.

    Functional brain correlates of social and nonsocial processes in autism spectrum disorders: an activation likelihood estimation meta-analysis

    Biol. Psychiatry

    (2009)
  • J. Duncan

    The multiple-demand (MD) system of the primate brain: mental programs for intelligent behaviour

    Trends Cognit. Sci.

    (2010)
  • J. Duncan

    The structure of cognition: attentional episodes in mind and brain

    Neuron

    (2013)
  • Cited by (18)

    • A Functional Magnetic Resonance Imaging Meta-Analysis of Childhood Trauma

      2024, Biological Psychiatry: Cognitive Neuroscience and Neuroimaging
    View all citing articles on Scopus
    View full text