Affective and physiological factors predicting maternal response to infant crying

https://doi.org/10.1016/j.infbeh.2008.10.005Get rights and content

Abstract

This study evaluated physiological, affective, and perceptual factors hypothesized to predict how quickly 45 primiparous mothers of 7–9-month-old infants would respond to non-distressed infant crying. Aversiveness ratings of the non-distressed cries of one's “own” infant and physiological reactivity to one's “own” infant crying accounted for a significant amount of the variance in a Cox proportional hazards regression analysis of speed of response. These findings suggest that mothers who have strong affective and physiological responses to non-distressed infant cries may be more likely to respond indiscriminately to attention-seeking infant behaviors.

Section snippets

Participants

To examine these hypotheses, 45 women who identified themselves as primiparous mothers of 7–9-month-old infants participated with their infants in this study. Mothers were recruited from the community via newspaper advertisements seeking first-time mothers of 7–9-month-old infants to participate in research in exchange for a free parenting workshop.

Procedure

Participation in the study required the mother and infant to visit the laboratory one time for approximately 2 h. After the mother completed a

Results

The sample was predominantly Caucasian (approximately 90%) and middle class (mean annual family income = $69,000). Mothers were generally well educated, with 80% finishing college. Of these mothers, approximately 24% worked part-time and 22% worked full-time. The infants consisted of 25 boys and 20 girls, with a mean age of 33 weeks. Means and standard deviations of the study variables are presented in Table 1. One-tailed correlations among independent variables are also presented in Table 1.

Of

Discussion

Infants exhibit different cries to signal their distressed and non-distressed internal states. Mild, non-distressed infant cries were the focus of inquiry here because in non-abusing, non-neglectful samples, mothers generally respond quickly to distressed cries (Gustafson & Harris, 1990); however, the speed with which they respond to non-distressed cries is what probably relates to increases in rates of infant crying (Belsky, 1984; Hubbard & van Ijzendoorn, 1991). Over time, the frequency of

Acknowledgement

This manuscript is based on the 2nd author's doctoral dissertation.

References (30)

  • J. Bernal

    Crying during the first 10 days of life and maternal responses

    Developmental Medicine and Child Neurology

    (1972)
  • C.F.Z. Boukydis et al.

    Adult physiological response to infant cries: Effects of temperament of infant, parental status, and gender

    Child Development

    (1982)
  • J. Cohen et al.
  • S. Crockenberg et al.

    Autonomy as competence in 2-year olds: Maternal correlates of child defiance, compliance, and self-assertion

    Developmental Psychology

    (1990)
  • M. De Wolff et al.

    Sensitivity and attachment: A meta-analysis on parental antecedents of infant attachment

    Child Development

    (1997)
  • Cited by (63)

    • Relationship between oxytocin and maternal approach behaviors to infants’ vocalizations

      2020, Comprehensive Psychoneuroendocrinology
      Citation Excerpt :

      Additionally, in women, administration of oxytocin was associated with reduced responses in the amygdala to crying [24] or laughing infants [25]. Further, it has been shown that the stronger caregivers experienced aversive feelings and heartrate responses to an infant’s crying, the quicker they responded [11]. Thus, we could hypothesize that caregivers with low oxytocin levels might respond to infants’ emotional vocalizations due to feeling stronger salience and anxiety toward the sounds.

    • Experimental manipulation of emotion regulation changes mothers’ physiological and facial expressive responses to infant crying

      2019, Infant Behavior and Development
      Citation Excerpt :

      Previous studies on emotion regulation have identified suppression as a maladaptive strategy. However, in a parenting context, arousal may stimulate the mother to respond promptly to comfort the infant (Del Vecchio et al., 2009). Physiological arousal may only impede sensitive caregiving responses when it is too strong (Joosen et al., 2013).

    • Putting salient vocalizations in context: Adults’ physiological arousal to emotive cues in domestic and external environments

      2018, Physiology and Behavior
      Citation Excerpt :

      Infant cry elicits general physiological arousal in parents and non-parents alike, and in both males and females [48,58]. Enhanced autonomic physiological reactions to infant vocalizations, including increases in skin conductance [44,48,147], heart rate, and blood pressure [17,29,31,44,47,53,141,142,147,151] have been documented. Cry-specific physiological responses, contrasting typical infant cry to modified vocalizations [39,40,125] and atypical cry [41,42], have also been demonstrated.

    View all citing articles on Scopus
    View full text