Elsevier

Early Childhood Research Quarterly

Volume 41, 4th Quarter 2017, Pages 84-94
Early Childhood Research Quarterly

Selective attention relates to the development of executive functions in 2,5- to 3-year-olds: A longitudinal study

https://doi.org/10.1016/j.ecresq.2017.06.005Get rights and content

Highlights

  • Preschool selective attention relates to working memory and inhibition development.

  • These links remain after accounting for SES, gender, verbal ability, and age.

  • Selective attention shows significant stability across a 6-months period.

Abstract

To study the central role of selective attention in the early development of executive functions (EFs), longitudinal relationships between selective attention, working memory, and simple response inhibition were explored. Selective attention, working memory, and simple response inhibition were assessed twice in our preschool sample (N = 273), which included a relatively large number of children from low SES families. The tasks were administered between age 2.5 (time 1) and 3 years (time 2). An analytical path model was tested to analyse the relationships simultaneously. The results indicate that selective attention at age 2.5 years predicts working memory and response inhibition at age 3 years. Controlling for gender, SES, home language, verbal ability, and age did not affect the strengths of these relationships.

Introduction

Executive Functions (EFs) refer to cognitive control processes aimed at regulating, organizing, and planning behaviour (Diamond & Lee, 2011) and have been found to be linked to good academic performance, good social skills, less criminal activity rates, and low substance abuse, or to overall success in life (Diamond, Barnett, Thomas, & Munro, 2007; Moffitt et al., 2011). Given the predictive validity of EF measures for such a wide range of developmental outcomes, EFs have increasingly become a focus point for early interventions. To target such interventions most optimally, it is essential that Executive Function (EF) development and its underlying factors are understood. However, although research on EF development at preschool age and beyond is flourishing, much less is known about EF development before the age of 3 years. Selective attention, or the ability to focus on a specific stimulus and to ignore other stimuli or distractors (Atkinson and Braddick, 2012, Mahone and Schneider, 2012; Plude, Enns, & Brodeur, 1994), has been hypothesized to constitute one of the core building blocks in infancy and toddlerhood on which (complex) EFs build as children grow older (Garon, Bryson, & Smith, 2008; Hendry, Jones, & Charman, 2016). Recent studies have indeed shown that measures of attention in infancy are predictive of EF in toddlerhood (Holmboe, Fearon, Csibra, Tucker, & Johnson, 2008; Johansson, Marciszko, Brocki, & Bohlin, 2015; Johansson, Marciszko, Gredebäck, Nyström, & Bohlin, 2015; Kochanska, Murray, & Harlan, 2000), providing initial evidence for the developmental model proposed by Garon et al. (2008) and Hendry, Jones, and Charman (2016). However, these previous studies were mostly small-sample laboratory studies with highly selective (i.e., high SES) groups of participants. As such, it is currently unknown whether results can be generalized to a wider population (including low SES children), and whether measures of selective attention remain of predictive value for EF development when assessed beyond infancy. This is particularly important, as intervention efforts aimed at boosting EF development, such as preschool remediation programmes, are likely to be targeted at low SES and/or immigrant children, and children often do not enroll into such programmes until some point during the 3rd year of life (for example, Department for Education, 2016; Government of the Netherlands, n.d.; U.S. Department of Health and Human Services, 2016). Therefore, the current study set out to investigate whether individual differences in selective attention at age 2.5 years, at a time when development of EF is particularly rapid (Gerardi-Caulton, 2000; Rueda, Posner, & Rothbart, 2005), predict EF at age 3 years in a large and heterogeneous sample. To this end, we used a previously validated battery of EF measures suitable for field-based assessment, focusing on core and relatively early emerging aspects of EF: inhibitory control and working memory (Mulder, Hoofs, Verhagen, Van der Veen, & Leseman, 2014). In the next sections, we first describe the general tenet of the hierarchical model of EF development, followed by a brief overview of current evidence regarding the relation between selective attention and early EF development.

A number of researchers (e.g., Bull, Espy, & Wiebe, 2008; Davidson, Amso, Anderson, & Diamond, 2006; Miyake, Friedman, Emerson, Witzki, & Howerter, 2000) agree that EF comprises three basic interrelated cognitive processes, namely: updating of information in working memory, inhibition of responses, and shifting of attention sets or response sets. The hierarchical model of EF development postulates that there is a certain ordering in the development of these skills, with relatively simple and more basic EF skills providing the foundation on which more complex skills are built (Garon et al., 2008), as will be briefly reviewed below.

A few days after birth, infants already demonstrate recognition memory, for example, the recognition of faces (Slater & Quinn, 2001). The ability to store and retrieve information over relatively short periods of time can be referred to as short-term memory (Astle and Scerif, 2011, Diamond, 2013). Memory span – the amount of visuospatial or phonological information that can be immediately recalled when it has been presented once – increases with age across childhood (Howard & Polich, 1985). Working memory involves both storing incoming visual or auditory information for brief periods of time, and actively performing cognitive operations on that information, such as updating or manipulation (Baddeley, 2003; Engel de Abreu, Conway, & Gathercole, 2010). As such, by definition, working memory relies on short-term memory. Working memory improves with age, from infancy through to adolescence and young adulthood (Diamond, Prevor, Callender, & Druin, 1997; Garon et al., 2008; Huizinga, Dolan, & Van der Molen, 2006).

Similarly, simple response inhibition, or the ability to refrain from acting on impulse, precedes development of more complex forms of inhibition (Garon et al., 2008). Simple response inhibition involves the suppression of a dominant response (Kochanska et al., 2000; Van der Ven, Kroesbergen, Boom, & Leseman, 2012). One example is the ability to delay gratification, where the dominant response – giving in to the temptation – has to be suppressed. Simple response inhibition improves gradually with age (for example, from 22 to 33 months: Kochanska et al., 2000; and from 33 months to 66 months: Kochanska, Murray, & Coy, 1997). Carlson (2005) also showed that the percentage of children who passed a simple response inhibition task (such as gift delay) increased significantly with age (from the age of 3 years to the age of 5 years). Complex response inhibition implies that children not only suppress their primary response but also replace it by a sub-dominant behaviour (Karreman, Van Tuijl, Van Aken, & Deković, 2008). This requires children to remember and initiate the appropriate behaviour. Thus, in complex response inhibition, not only inhibition but also working memory is important. Like simple response inhibition, complex response inhibition improves with age. For example, Carlson (2005) showed that the percentage of children who passed a complex inhibition task (Stroop tasks such as “Grass/Snow” and “Bear/Dragon”) increased significantly from the age of 3 years to the age of 5 years.

Shifting involves the ability to change flexibly between different tasks or between using different rules within the same task (Miyake et al., 2000). Shifting involves both working memory and inhibitory control processes (Blakey, Visser, & Caroll, 2016). Due to this relatively high level of complexity, children do not start to pass standard shifting tasks such as the Dimensional Card Sorting Task until after age 3 years (Zelazo, 2006). Although recent studies have started to include more basic shifting tasks for younger children too (for an overview, see Garon et al., 2008), these involve primarily lab-based measures which cannot easily be applied in field-based research. Similarly, at the time of the design of the current study, complex inhibition measures suitable for large-scale field-based research in toddlerhood were not available. As such, the current study is focused on the two core aspects of early EF: working memory and simple response inhibition.

The first 3 years of life are marked by rapid development of the ability to selectively attend to stimuli and ignore distracting information (Gerhardstein & Rovee-Collier, 2002; Scerif, Cornish, Wilding, Driver, & Karmiloff-Smith, 2004; Posner & Rothbart, 2007), and individual differences appear to be, at least partly, stable over time from the second half of the first year of life to 2.5 years of age (Kannass, Oakes, & Shaddy, 2006). In their developmental models, Garon et al. (2008) and Hendry et al. (2016) suggest that attention, and selective attention in particular, may provide one of the first ‘building blocks’ for, or precursors to, emerging EF, such as working memory and inhibitory control. Subsequently, across childhood and beyond, selective attention is assumed to continue to play a direct and important role in EF task performance (cf. Hendry et al., 2016), as regulating, organizing, and planning behaviour all involve attention.

A few longitudinal studies have shown that individual differences in infant selective attention predict EFs in toddlerhood. First, significant predictive relations from attention assessed in infancy to inhibitory control and working memory in toddlerhood have been observed (Holmboe et al., 2008, Johansson, Marciszko, Gredebäck et al., 2015). Moreover, Johansson, Marciszko, Brocki et al. (2015) found that infant attention predicted working memory at age 3 years. However, these studies all included small and mostly selected high SES samples with laboratory assessments. One exception is a study by Kochanska et al. (2000), which showed that observed attention in infancy predicted performance on a battery of inhibitory control measures at 22, but not 33 months in a sample of mixed SES backgrounds. Thus, there is converging evidence that infant attention serves as one of the core building blocks of emerging EF. However, replication in large heterogeneous samples is needed, and it is currently unclear whether selective attention remains a unique predictor of EF development when assessed beyond infancy. The present study aimed to address these issues, by studying if and how selective attention predicts EFs towards the beginning of the preschool period, including a large and diverse sample. Specifically, predictive relations from selective attention at age 2.5 years (time 1) to working memory and inhibitory control 6 months later, at age 3 years (time 2) were studied. Before turning to the goals of the present study, the theoretical rationale, including underlying mechanisms linking selective attention and EF, will be discussed. Furthermore, experimental work with older children and adults which provides evidence regarding the underlying mechanisms through which selective attention and EFs are linked, will be described.

The central underlying mechanism linking selective attention and EFs is that an important function of selective attention is the ability to resolve conflicts among thoughts, feelings, and responses, whereas resolving conflicts is crucial for EF performance as well (Garon et al., 2008, Posner and Rothbart, 2007). Hence, orienting on relevant stimuli while ignoring other (possibly very attractive but,) irrelevant stimuli increases both working memory performance and inhibitory control. In addition, selective attentional orienting has been shown to aid in regulating and controlling emotions in young children (Harman, Rothbart, & Posner, 1997; Posner, Rothbart, Sheese, & Voelker, 2012; Rothbart, Sheese, Rueda, & Posner, 2011).

With regard to working memory, selective and focused attention is important in all stages: encoding relevant stimuli, holding the mental representation active in mind during the delay, retrieving relevant stimuli, making decisions, and responding. In the presence of several distracting stimuli, encoding relevant stimuli is particularly dependent of selective attention: the ability to focus attention and ignore distractors (Gazzaley & Nobre, 2012). With regard to inhibitory control, Mischel and colleagues propose that orienting attention on other stimuli or distractors (referred to as self-distraction) serves as a strategy to reduce the difficulty of inhibiting behaviour (Metcalfe & Mischel, 1999; Mischel, Cantor, & Feldman, 1996; Mischel, Shoda, & Rodriguez, 1989; Sethi, Mischel, Aber, Shoda, & Rodriguez, 2000). In accordance, different EF tasks may draw upon selective attention skills in a different way. In particular, a key distinction occurs between tasks which require focusing of attention on the task material, such as working memory tasks, and tasks which require moving the task material out of focus in order to reduce its emotional saliency, such as during delay of gratification tasks used to tap inhibitory control. Hence, focused attention is suggested to be essential for working memory task performance, whereas distracting attention is propounded to be important in order to succeed in delay of gratification tasks.

Finally, a distinction could be made between tasks relying on dorsolateral frontal cortex, involved in cognitive processes (cool EF), and tasks relying more on emotion and motivation and in which the orbitofrontal cortex is involved (hot EF), for example delay of gratification tasks (Alvarez and Emory, 2006, Zelazo and Müller, 2002). Likewise, the distinction between cool EF and hot EF has been demonstrated in studies in which EF tasks were administered to young children (Bassett, Denham, Wyatt, & Warren-Khot, 2012; Brock, Rimm-Kaufman, Nathanson, & Grimm, 2009; Willoughby, Kupersmidt, Voegler-Lee, & Bryant, 2011).

Next, current evidence from experimental studies regarding the role of selective attention in working memory and inhibitory control, measured through delay tasks, will be discussed.

Selective attention has been consistently linked to short-term memory capacity in adults (Poole & Kane, 2009; Vogel, McCollough, & Machizawa, 2005; Zanto & Gazzaley, 2009). Moreover, several neurophysiological studies into the association between selective attention and working memory have shown that the same brain regions are involved in both selective attention and working memory in adulthood (Awh, Anllo-Vento, & Hillyard, 2000; Awh, Smith, & Jonides, 1995; Kiyonaga & Egner, 2013; Mayer et al., 2007; Rutman, Clapp, Chadick, & Gazzaley, 2010; Sreenivasan & Jha, 2007; Zanto, Rubens, Thangavel, & Gazzaley, 2011). A recent series of experimental studies with infants suggests that the close ties between selective attention and memory are already present during the first year of life (Markant, Ackerman, Nussenbaum, & Amso, 2016; Markant and Amso, 2013, Markant and Amso, 2016; Ross-Sheehy, Oakes, & Luck, 2011). For example, Markant and Amso (2013) showed that selective attentional suppression of a previously visited location facilitated memory encoding of a target appearing at a new location in 9-month-old infants. Moreover, work with older children, aged 7 and 10 years, and adults shows evidence that cueing of selective attentional orienting towards the location of a target which needs to be retained in short-term memory facilitates memory performance. Most importantly, individual differences in sensitivity to attentional cueing predicted performance on a standard measure of working memory (Astle, Nobre, & Scerif, 2012). Thus, these experimental studies combined provide evidence that selective attention and working memory are closely tied from infancy through to childhood and beyond. In particular, the strength of encoding of information in working memory appears to be strongly reliant on both selective attentional orienting to the relevant stimulus and the ability to ignore irrelevant distracting information at the same time.

Associations between selective attention and inhibitory control, as assessed with delay of gratification tasks, have been consistently found in young children (Peake, Mischel, & Hebl, 2002; Rodriquez, Mischel, & Shoda, 1989; Sethi et al., 2000; Vaughn, Kopp, Krakow, Johnson, & Schwartz, 1986). Importantly, Mischel and colleagues have shown a direct effect of effective attention deployment on children’s ability to delay gratification, both in low-risk 4- to-5-year olds (Peake et al., 2002), and in maladjusted 6- to 12-year-old boys (Rodriquez et al., 1989), such that active selective attentional distraction strategies – that is, persistent distraction of attention away from the reward – facilitate performance. There is some evidence that relations between selective attentional distraction strategies and inhibitory control on delay of gratification tasks are relatively stable over developmental time. For example Sethi et al. (2000), used a prospective longitudinal study on attentional precursors of delay abilities in preschoolers from primarily upper- to middle-class families. They found that 18-months-old toddlers who were able to use distraction as a coping strategy during a separation from their mother in a structured laboratory situation were better able to wait in the standard delay paradigm at age 5 than toddlers who were unable to direct their attention away during mother’s absence. Thus, previous studies have shown that direction of attentional focus during delay of gratification tasks aids inhibitory control in children.

The aim of the present study was to examine the role selective attention plays in the development of EFs in a heterogeneous sample of 2–3-year-old children, using a longitudinal design. Based on the perspective that EF is at least partly componential (Miyake et al., 2000) and the fact that the developmental timing of various EF abilities varies (Carlson, 2005; Garon et al., 2008; Klenberg, Korman, & Lahti-Nuuttila, 2001), the interrelations among selective attention and two distinct EFs (simple response inhibition and working memory) are examined in 2–3-year-old children. By doing so, this study is, to our knowledge, the first to evaluate the relation between selective attention in toddlerhood and developing EFs in a mixed sample involving a large number of disadvantaged children, focusing on two key aspects of early EF – working memory and simple response inhibition – which are both hypothesized to recruit selective attention, albeit through different underlying mechanisms (i.e., focusing and distracting). Given previous experimental and longitudinal findings, we hypothesize that selective attention at age 2.5 years uniquely predicts working memory and simple response inhibition at age 3 years (see Fig. 1). To establish independent predictive relations between selective attention at time 1 and EF at time 2, we included the autocorrelations between time 1 and time 2 working memory and time 1 and time 2 response inhibition in the model. Moreover, we controlled for time 1 short-term memory in the prediction of time 2 working memory, given the importance of short-term memory development for working memory in the hierarchical model (Garon et al., 2008). In addition, we explored the developmental stability of individual differences in selective attention across this period. Based on previous work that individual differences in attention already show some stability from infancy onwards (Kannass et al., 2006), we hypothesized that selective attention would show significant stability over time.

Section snippets

Design

A longitudinal design was used in this study, with measurements at two occasions (time 1 and time 2). One group of participants was recruited in the fall of 2012 for the first measurement, and their second measurement occasion was in the spring of 2013. A second group of participants was recruited in the fall of 2013, and their second measurement occasion was in the spring of 2014. We chose to limit the time span between the two measurement occasions to approximately 6 months (but always after

Results

The nested model with the hypothesized relations between selective attention and EFs (model 1a) provided acceptable fit for the data: CFI = .977, TLI = .942, RMSEA = .056, SRMR = .032, and a X2(6) of 11.091 (p = .0856). The unnested model with the hypothesized relations between selective attention and EFs (model 1b) provided a better fit for the data: CFI = .980, TLI = .951, RMSEA = .054, SRMR = .032, and a X2(6) of 10.722 (p = .0973). Including the background variables successively in additional models (models

Discussion

In the current study, a heterogeneous sample of children was recruited and followed from age 2.5 to 3 years to explore the central role of selective attention in the development of EFs. The relations between selective attention, working memory, and simple response inhibition were examined using path analysis. The findings clearly showed that selective attention at age 2.5 years is uniquely related to the development of the two EFs over time. Controlling for gender, home language, SES, age, and

Acknowledgements

The current study was supported by the Ministry of Education, Culture, and Science in the Netherlands. The sponsor had no involvement in the study design, data collection, analysis, and interpretation of data, or other aspects of the present study. The authors want to thank all parents, children, and preschool teachers for their participation in this study.

References (102)

  • J. Markant et al.

    Selective attention neutralizes the adverse effects of low socioeconomic status on memory in 9-month-old infants

    Developmental Cognitive Neuroscience

    (2016)
  • J.S. Mayer et al.

    Common neural substrates for visual working memory and attention

    NeuroImage

    (2007)
  • A. Miyake et al.

    The unity and diversity of executive functions and their contributions to complex “frontal lobe” tasks: A latent variable analysis

    Cognitive Psychology

    (2000)
  • O. Oudgenoeg-Paz et al.

    Development of exploration of spatial-relational object properties in the second and third years of life

    Journal of Experimental Child Psychology

    (2016)
  • D.J. Plude et al.

    The development of selective attention: A life-span overview

    Acta Psychologica

    (1994)
  • S.A. Rose et al.

    Information processing in toddlers: Continuity from infancy and persistence from preterm deficits

    Intelligence

    (2009)
  • A. Von Suchodoletz et al.

    Behavioral self-regulation and relations to emergent academic skills among children in Germany and Iceland

    Early Childhood Research Quarterly

    (2013)
  • J.A. Alvarez et al.

    Executive Function and the frontal lobes: A meta-analytic review

    Neuropsychology Review

    (2006)
  • D.E. Astle et al.

    Attentional control constrains visual short-term memory: Insights from developmental and individual differences

    The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology

    (2012)
  • J. Atkinson et al.

    Visual attention in the first years: Typical development and developmental disorders

    Developmental Medicine and Child Neurology

    (2012)
  • E. Awh et al.

    Human rehearsal processes and the frontal cortex: PET evidence

  • E. Awh et al.

    The role of spatial selective attention in working memory for locations: Evidence from event-related potentials

    Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience

    (2000)
  • A. Baddeley

    Working memory: Looking back and looking forward

    Neuroscience

    (2003)
  • H.H. Bassett et al.

    Refining the preschool self-regulation assessment for use in preschool classrooms

    Infant and Child Development

    (2012)
  • E. Bialystok et al.

    Bilingual minds

    Psychological Science in the Public Interest

    (2009)
  • E. Bialystok

    Bilingualism in development: Language, literacy, and cognition

    (2001)
  • C. Blair et al.

    Relating effortful control, executive function, and false belief understanding to emerging math and literacy ability in kindergarten

    Child Development

    (2007)
  • E. Blakey et al.

    Different executive functions support different kinds of cognitive flexibility: Evidence from 2-, 3-, and 4-year-olds

    Child Development

    (2016)
  • R. Bull et al.

    Short-term memory, working memory, and executive functioning in preschoolers: Longitudinal predictors of mathematical achievement at age 7 years

    Developmental Neuropsychology

    (2008)
  • S.M. Carlson et al.

    Bilingual experience and executive functioning in young children

    Developmental Science

    (2008)
  • S.M. Carlson

    Developmentally sensitive measures of executive function in preschool children

    Developmental Neuropsychology

    (2005)
  • C.A.C. Clark et al.

    Preschool executive functioning abilities predict early mathematics achievement

    Developmental Psychology

    (2010)
  • Department for Education

    Provision for children under 5 years of age in England, January 2016 (SFR 23/2016)

    (2016)
  • A. Diamond et al.

    Interventions shown to aid executive function development in children 4–12 years old

    Science

    (2011)
  • A. Diamond et al.

    Prefrontal cortex cognitive deficits in children treated early and continuously for PKU

    Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development

    (1997)
  • A. Diamond et al.

    Preschool program improves cognitive control

    Science

    (2007)
  • A. Diamond

    Executive functions

    The Annual Review of Psychology

    (2013)
  • M. Dunn et al.

    Peabody picture vocabulary test-III-NL

    (2005)
  • P.M.J. Engel de Abreau et al.

    Working memory and fluid intelligence in young children

    Intelligence

    (2010)
  • M.E. Ensminger et al.

    A decade of measuring SES: What it tells us and where to go from here

  • N. Garon et al.

    Executive function in preschoolers: A review using an integrative framework

    Psychological Bulletin

    (2008)
  • N. Garon et al.

    A novel executive function test battery for pre-schoolers: Sensitivity to age differences

    Child Neuropsychology

    (2013)
  • G. Gerardi-Caulton

    Sensitivity to spatial conflict and the development of self-regulation in children 24–36 months of age

    Developmental Science

    (2000)
  • Government of the Netherlands

    Early childhood education (VVE)

    (2016)
  • C. Harman et al.

    Distress and attention interactions in early infancy

    Motivation and Emotion

    (1997)
  • L. Howard et al.

    P300 latency and memory span development

    Developmental Psychology

    (1985)
  • J.J. Hox

    Multilevel analysis: Techniques and applications

    (2002)
  • L. Hu et al.

    Cutoff criteria for fit indexes in covariance structure analysis: Conventional criteria versus new alternatives

    Structural Equation Modeling: A Multidisciplinary Journal

    (1999)
  • C. Hughes

    Executive functions and development: Emerging themes

    Infant and Child Development

    (2002)
  • M. Johansson et al.

    Individual differences in early executive functions: A longitudinal study from 12 to 36 months

    Infant and Child Development

    (2015)
  • Cited by (0)

    View full text