Elsevier

Consciousness and Cognition

Volume 28, August 2014, Pages 94-103
Consciousness and Cognition

Cultural background influences implicit but not explicit sense of agency for the production of musical tones

https://doi.org/10.1016/j.concog.2014.06.013Get rights and content

Highlights

  • We examined the effect outcome pleasantness on the sense of agency (SoA).

  • We recruited western and non-western participants.

  • Westerners showed stronger intentional binding (IB) for pleasant outcomes.

  • Both groups reported stronger feeling of control (FoC) for pleasant outcomes.

  • For IB the effect of outcome pleasantness is determined by cultural differences.

Abstract

The sense of agency is suggested to occur at both low and high levels by the involvement of sensorimotor processes and the contribution of retrospective inferences based on contextual cues. In the current study, we recruited western and non-western participants and examined the effect of pleasantness of action outcomes on both feeling of control ratings and intentional binding which refers to the perceived compression of the temporal delay between actions and outcomes. We found that both western and non-western groups showed greater feeling of control ratings for the consonant (pleasant) compared to dissonant (unpleasant) outcomes. The intentional binding effect, on the other hand, was stronger for the consonant compared to dissonant outcomes in the western group only. We discuss the results in relation to how cultural background might differentially influence the effect of outcome pleasantness on low and high levels of the sense of agency.

Introduction

The sense of agency refers to the subjective experience that accompanies our actions; it is the sense that one has control over their actions and their consequent outcomes. Recent research has suggested that the nature of the mechanism behind how we experience the sense of agency is not only driven by the internal/sensorimotor processes but also influenced by environmental and contextual factors.

It has been proposed, for example, that agentic experience might occur at two levels (Synofzik, Vosgerau, & Newen, 2008). The low level feeling of agency is described as a pre-reflective (Gallagher, 2000) and non-conceptual experience of agency. This low level sense of agency is proposed to emerge from sensorimotor processes (Synofzik et al., 2008), including the matching between predictions produced by an internal forward model and actual consequences of actions (Blakemore et al., 2002, Frith, 2005, Frith et al., 2000). At a higher level, the agentic experience emerges as an explicit or reflective judgment which is driven by inferences, beliefs, and contextual cues (Bayne and Pacherie, 2007, Moore et al., 2012, Obhi and Hall, 2011a, Synofzik et al., 2008, Wegner, 2003, Wegner, 2004, Wegner and Wheatley, 1999).

In experimental settings, the sense of agency has been measured by obtaining self reports of participants’ subjective feeling of control over their actions or outcomes (Balslev et al., 2007, Ebert and Wegner, 2011, Linser and Goschke, 2007, Metcalfe and Greene, 2007, Sato and Yasuda, 2005, Sebanz and Lackner, 2007, Wegner and Wheatley, 1999, Wenke et al., 2010). Alternatively, the intentional binding effect has been used as an indirect measure of the sense of agency (Barlas and Obhi, 2013, Haggard et al., 2002, Haggard et al., 2009, Moore et al., 2009, Obhi and Hall, 2011a, Obhi and Hall, 2011b, Obhi et al., 2013, Wohlschläger et al., 2003). Intentional binding refers to the perceived attraction between the timing of actions and the onset of their consequences (Haggard et al., 2002), and has been suggested to index the experience of agency (see Moore & Obhi, 2012, for a review on intentional binding).

Recent findings have suggested that the intentional binding effect can be influenced by several contextual and situational factors. It has been shown, for example, that the intentional binding effect was reduced when memories involving in depressive life periods are activated compared to the recollection of relatively neutral events (Obhi et al., 2013). In another study, it has been shown that causal beliefs regarding the source of the actions resulted in the stronger binding effect when participants believed that they produced the outcomes compared to when they believed that another person was the cause (Desantis, Roussel, & Waszak, 2011).

One important question regarding the agentic experience concerns situations where actions generate outcomes that differ in their perceived pleasantness. Recent studies have shown, for example, that negative action outcomes had an attenuating effect on intentional binding compared to positive or neutral outcomes (Takahata et al., 2012, Yoshie and Haggard, 2013). Similarly, priming participants with positive pictures compared to neutral ones was found to increase the intentional binding effect (Aarts et al., 2012).

A critical extension to this question is whether perceived pleasantness can affect the sense of agency differentially based on potential cultural variations. In the current study, we targeted at this question and examined how intentional binding and the explicit feeling of control over action outcomes would be influenced when these outcomes differed in terms of their perceived pleasantness, which is potentially shaped by cultural differences.

As action outcomes, we used consonant and dissonant piano chords that have long been subject to the study of music perception by the different sensations they evoke in listeners. According to the Pythagorean view, the relative simplicity of the frequency ratio of two tones played simultaneously determines the pleasantness of the outcome sound (Helmholtz, 1877, Tenney, 1988). Consonance, in this regard, refers to the pleasantness produced by the co-occurrence of two tones whereas dissonance is described as unpleasant due to the beating and roughness (Dell’Acqua et al., 2006, Dellacherie et al., 2011, Plantinga and Trehub, 2013, Shapira Lots and Stone, 2008). The major view regarding the perception of these tonal structures suggests that the stability and pleasant-sounding attributes make consonance preferred over instable and rough-sounding dissonance (Bidelman and Krishnan, 2009, McDermott and Hauser, 2004).

However, the issue regarding the relationship between psychological and neurophysiological basis of consonance preference and its universal prevalence remains unresolved. One contention is that preference for consonance is innate and is due to certain constraints in the auditory system (Schellenberg and Trehub, 1996, Tramo et al., 2001). In support of this view, studies with infants measuring their looking-time preference suggests that infants as young as 2 and 4-month-olds (Trainor, Tsang, & Cheung, 2002) and 2-day-olds (Masataka, 2006) prefer to listen to consonant excerpts over dissonant ones. However, there is also accumulating evidence suggesting that consonant preference is the product of learning mechanisms. Vassilakis (2005), for example, examined Middle Eastern, North Indian, and Bosnian musical structures and noted that beats, which are thought to reside in dissonance, are well accepted in the musical structure of these cultures. In addition, Plantinga and Trehub (2013) tested consonance preference among 6-year-old infants and found that the listening time to the consonant chords was not longer than dissonant ones. Moreover, they showed that after a 3-min exposure to either consonant or dissonant stimuli, infants listened to the familiar intervals longer, regardless of their consonant or dissonant status.

The current study takes into account both lines of findings suggesting enhanced sense of agency over positive outcomes and cultural variances in the perceived pleasantness of consonance to address two important questions. First, these two types of stimuli would allow us to investigate whether low and high levels of the sense of agency are similarly affected by the pleasantness of action outcomes. Second, as consonance preference is suggested to vary across different cultures (e.g. Vassilakis, 2005), our design could reveal whether this variance can manifest itself on either low or high levels of the sense of agency.

In the current study, participants completed a computer based task in which they made a voluntary right or left key press which was followed by either consonant or dissonant piano chords. We determined the intentional binding effect, subjective feelings of control over the chords, and participants’ ratings for how much they liked each of consonant and dissonant chords.

Based on the common bias towards attribution of the self as a cause of positive outcomes (Campbell & Sedikides, 1999), we predicted that the perceived pleasantness of consonant chords would produce higher feelings of control and liking ratings as well as stronger binding effect (Yoshie & Haggard, 2013) compared to the dissonant ones. As consonant and dissonant chords are based specifically on western tonal structure, our second prediction was that we would observe a greater effect of consonance in the western group compared to the non-western group.

Section snippets

Participants

In total, thirty-four right-handed participants were recruited from the participant pool of Wilfrid Laurier University. The study was approved by the Research Ethics Board of Wilfrid Laurier University, and all participants gave written informed consent prior to beginning the study. We excluded four participants who, in at least one condition, had their mean judgment errors two standard deviations away from the sample mean. In addition, the data of one participant who could not follow the

Results

We excluded the trials with key press response times shorter than 600 ms and with time judgment errors being three standard deviations away from participant’s average judgment error. In addition, trials in which participants made the second key press during the clock rotation different than the one on the previous step were removed from the data. The exclusion criteria resulted in the removal of 3.4% of all trials (range: 0.7–7.5%).

Discussion

In the current study, we examined the effect of perceived pleasantness of the action outcomes on both intentional binding and subjective judgments of agency in western and non-western participants. We found that both groups felt significantly more control over the consonant chords than the dissonant ones and gave higher ratings of liking the former than the latter. The low level sense of agency indexed by the intentional binding effect was influenced by chord type in the western group only.

Acknowledgment

This work was made possible by an NSERC discovery Grant awarded to SSO.

References (48)

  • J.W. Moore et al.

    Intentional binding and the sense of agency: A review

    Consciousness and Cognition

    (2012)
  • J.W. Moore et al.

    Modulating the sense of agency with external cues

    Consciousness and Cognition

    (2009)
  • A. Sato et al.

    Illusion of sense of self-agency: Discrepancy between the predicted and actual sensory consequences of actions modulates the sense of self-agency, but not the sense of self-ownership

    Cognition

    (2005)
  • N. Sebanz et al.

    Who’s calling the shots? Intentional content and feelings of control

    Consciousness and Cognition

    (2007)
  • M. Synofzik et al.

    Beyond the comparator model: A multifactorial two-step account of agency

    Consciousness and Cognition

    (2008)
  • D.M. Wegner

    The mind’s best trick: How we experience conscious will

    Trends in Cognitive Sciences

    (2003)
  • D. Wenke et al.

    Subliminal priming of actions influences sense of control over effects of action

    Cognition

    (2010)
  • H. Aarts et al.

    Positive priming and intentional binding: Eye-blink rate predicts reward information effects on the sense of agency

    Social Neuroscience

    (2012)
  • D. Balslev et al.

    Proprioception contributes to the sense of agency during visual observation of hand movements: Evidence from temporal judgments of action

    Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience

    (2007)
  • Z. Barlas et al.

    Freedom, choice, and the sense of agency

    Frontiers in Human Neuroscience

    (2013)
  • T. Bayne et al.

    Narrators and comparators: The architecture of agentive self-awareness

    Synthese

    (2007)
  • G.M. Bidelman et al.

    Neural correlates of consonance, dissonance, and the hierarchy of musical pitch in the human brainstem

    The Journal of Neuroscience: The Official Journal of the Society for Neuroscience

    (2009)
  • W. Campbell et al.

    Self-threat magnifies the self-serving bias: A meta-analytic integration

    Review of General Psychology

    (1999)
  • D. Dellacherie et al.

    The effect of musical experience on emotional self-reports and psychophysiological responses to dissonance

    Psychophysiology

    (2011)
  • Cited by (24)

    • Social, affective, and non-motoric bodily cues to the Sense of Agency: A systematic review of the experience of control

      2022, Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews
      Citation Excerpt :

      A more consistent pattern of results was seemingly observed with respect to the hedonic value of outcomes, with stronger explicit SoA for pleasant vs. unpleasant sounds. However, their effects on implicit SoA were inconsistent, with one study reporting enhanced implicit SoA for pleasant vs. unpleasant outcomes (Barlas and Obhi, 2014), another one reporting similar effects on action binding (Tanaka and Kawabata, 2021), and two that did not identify any modulation (Barlas et al., 2017, 2018). Hence, whether the positive or negative valence of (unrewarded) outcomes may modulate SoA remains unclear.

    • To follow or not to follow: Influence of valence and consensus on the sense of agency

      2022, Consciousness and Cognition
      Citation Excerpt :

      Therefore, they were presumably quite confident about making the correct choice, wherefore in this case, the negative outcome would be incongruent to the anticipated result which has been reported to decrease implicit agency (Barlas & Kopp, 2018; Blakemore et al., 1999; Sato & Yasuda, 2005). However, as unexpected outcomes (provided that participants are confident about their choice, i.e., choosing the wrong cup) were found to increase temporal binding (Majchrowicz & Wierzchoń, 2018) and predictably positive outcomes decreased temporal binding in a prior study (Christensen et al., 2016; but see also Barlas & Obhi, 2014), it seems likely that increased temporal binding with unpredicted negative outcomes is due to the valence of the outcome rather than the fact that it was unpredicted. Additionally, consensus did not have an influence on the perceived shortening of the interval between action and outcome.

    • When robots tell you what to do: Sense of agency in human- and robot-guided actions

      2019, Consciousness and Cognition
      Citation Excerpt :

      The tendency to attribute positive as opposed to negative events to one’s actions complies with the well-known notion of self-serving bias (Duval & Silvia, 2002; Miller & Ross, 1975; Taylor & Brown, 1994). In this vein, the current findings strengthen the proposed link between SoA and self-serving bias (Barlas & Obhi, 2014; Yoshie & Haggard, 2013) in the context of unpredictable outcomes produced by free and instructed actions. The effect of outcome-valence on explicit control ratings also complies with the previous studies that reported stronger control ratings over pleasant compared to unpleasant auditory outcomes (Barlas et al., 2017, 2018).

    View all citing articles on Scopus
    View full text