Elsevier

Cognition

Volume 195, February 2020, 104131
Cognition

Who's got the global advantage? Visual field differences in processing of global and local shape

https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cognition.2019.104131Get rights and content

Abstract

Much evidence suggests that we first perceive the overall layout of a scene or object followed later by the details. This coarse-to-fine temporal dynamic in visual processing is also found in Navon's classical paradigm where information at the global level of compound stimuli is processed faster than information at the local level (global precedence effect), and where information at the global level has larger effects on local level responses than local level information has on global level responses (asymmetric interference effects). Traditionally, global shape primacy in Navon's paradigm has been linked with a right hemisphere preference (left visual field advantage) for global shape processing, and a left hemisphere preference (right visual field advantage) for local shape processing. This link, however, has been based on measures which confound global precedence and interference effects. Indeed, when these measures are de-confounded, we find no evidence for larger global precedence effects in the left compared with the right visual field in a large sample of participants (N = 337). In comparison, global-to-local interference effects are found to be stronger in the left than in the right visual field. We argue that these findings can be accounted for by assuming that the right hemisphere plays a special role in integrating shape information across spatial scales, that is, without assuming the existence of a right hemisphere preference for global shape processing per se.

Introduction

The idea that the cerebral hemispheres perform different functions was expressed already when Broca (1865) concluded that “We speak with the left hemisphere”. It took longer to appreciate the right hemisphere's modus operandi, and for many years it was simply considered minor or non-dominant (Ivry & Robertson, 1998). Even though the notion of major/minor hemisphere is abandoned today, the division of labor between the hemispheres is still debated. A functional difference that has caught on since it was first introduced in the 1960s (Levy-Agresti & Sperry, 1968) is the global/local (holistic/analytical) dichotomy according to which the right hemisphere (RH) is specialized for processing of global information, e.g. the overall layout of a scene, and the left hemisphere (LH) for processing of local information, e.g. details/parts.

A paradigm that has become central to studies of the global/local dichotomy is the one that Navon used in 1977 to answer the question: “Do we perceive a visual scene feature-by-feature? Or is the process instantaneous and simultaneous as some Gestalt psychologists believed?” (p. 353). Navon's paradigm is based on presentation of compound stimuli (Asch, 1962); usually a large letter (global level) composed of smaller letters (local level) in which the global and the local letters can be the same (consistent) or different (inconsistent) (see also Kinchla (1974) and Pomerantz and Sager (1975)). Navon made two observations with this paradigm which he argued supported “…the notion that global processing is a necessary stage of perception prior to more fine-grained analysis” (p. 371): (i) responses were faster to the global than the local level, and (ii) when the levels were inconsistent, information at the global level interfered with (slowed down) responses to the local level, but not the other way around.

Not long after, Martin (1979) used compound letters in a divided visual field paradigm to examine: (i) if global level responses were more efficient when stimuli were presented in the left visual field (VF) (RH) than when presented in the right VF (LH), and (ii) whether local level responses were more efficient when stimuli were presented in the right compared with the left VF. She found evidence for both propositions but argued that the right VF advantage for local level responses was stronger than the left VF advantage for global level responses. Martin's findings with normal subjects were soon confirmed in split-brain patients (Delis, Kramer, & Kiefner, 1988; Robertson, Lamb, & Zaidel, 1993), in patients with unilateral brain damage (Robertson & Lamb, 1991), and in functional imaging with normal subjects (Fink et al., 1996). Despite this convergence, VF differences in normal subjects have nevertheless proven rather flimsy being reported in some studies but not others (Blanca & Lopez-Montiel, 2009; Van Kleeck, 1989; Yovel, Levy, & Yovel, 2001), and it is still debated what might be the cause of these inconsistencies (Aiello et al., 2018; Flevaris & Robertson, 2016).

One obvious explanation may be that the VF effects captured with Navon's paradigm are small, and that failures to find them could reflect low statistical power (Lamb & Yund, 1996; Martens & Hübner, 2013). Given that most studies in this area are based on small samples (N < 30) this is not unlikely, and the magnitude of the effects when observed may thus also have been overestimated (Button et al., 2013; Open Science Collaboration, 2015). Another explanation is that different indexes can be derived from Navon's paradigm, and that these indexes do not necessarily measure the same aspects of global/local processing (Gerlach & Krumborg, 2014). Hence, there is ample evidence suggesting that what Navon originally referred to as global precedence actually reflects two independent effects: a global precedence effect and a global-to-local interference effect (Hübner & Volberg, 2005; Lamb & Yund, 1996; Robertson et al., 1993). More specifically, it has been suggested that the magnocellular pathway is responsible for the global precedence effect (Poirel et al., 2011; Poirel, Pineau, & Mellet, 2008), whereas the global-to-local interference effect rests upon automatic identification processes: Interference is observed as soon as a different identifiable stimulus is present at the global level during local level processing (Beaucousin et al., 2011). Consequently, during a global-local paradigm with compound letters such as the one used by Navon (1977), the identifiable distractor present at the global level is automatically processed and must be inhibited to perform correctly at the local level, leading to the global-to-local interference effect (Poirel et al., 2014).

Even though the global-local dichotomy has typically been associated with hemispheric differences, it has also been suggested that global information may be processed more efficiently in the lower than the upper VF whereas the reverse may be true for local information (Previc, 1990). Upper/lower VF differences have been found in many domains (Thomas & Elias, 2011), and may be linked to ventral and dorsal stream processing respectively (Milner & Goodale, 2006). Specific support for the notion that global/local processing may differ between the upper and lower VFs was reported by Christman (1993) who found a lower VF advantage for global level responses and an upper VF advantage for local level responses to inconsistent compound letters (experiment 1). Despite being larger than the left/right VF differences also found, the lower VF advantage for global level responses could not be replicated in his second experiment. Nevertheless, we concur with Christman (1993) “…that researchers studying left-right visual field differences in perceptual processing will need to address their relation to similar upper-lower visual field differences” (p. 278). Thus, the objective of the present study was to examine differences in global/local processing in all VFs (left/right & upper/lower) using compound letters. To address the shortcomings of previous studies, we examined a large sample (N = 337) by means of two indexes which tap global precedence and global-to-local interference effects reliably and independently. Based on the literature considered above both indexes were expected to yield higher values in the left than the right VF and in the lower compared with the upper VF.

Section snippets

Participants

400 first-year psychology students, naïve to the specific hypotheses tested, took part in the study as part of their course in cognitive psychology. The course is approved by the study board at the Department of Psychology, University of Southern Denmark, and the experiments conducted do not require formal ethical approval/registration according to Danish Law and the institutional requirements. Prior to participation the students were informed that data collected in the experiments might be

Check of effects and assumptions

As can be seen from Table 1, both the global precedence effect and the global-to-local interference effect were credible (their 95% CI did not contain 0) and of similar magnitudes (average d = 0.58 and d = 0.59). In comparison, the local-to-global interference effect was much smaller (average d = 0.14) albeit also credible. Hence, the typical global precedence, interference and interlevel interference effects were obtained. However, as can be seen from the histograms presented in Fig. 2a–d, the

Discussion

The present findings suggest that the most widely used index in laterality studies based on Navon's paradigm–the contrast between global inconsistent and local inconsistent trials–is confounded. Hence, contrary to what has been implicitly assumed, this index does not necessarily reflect interference effects alone. In fact, most of the variance associated with it may, as we show here, originate from global precedence effects. Consequently, use of this contrast should probably be avoided. The

Funding

This work was supported by a grant from the Danish Research Council for the Humanities (DFF – 4001-00115) to CG.

Acknowledgements

We wish to express our gratitude to the Friends of Fakutsi Association (FFA), and Richard Ivry for discussions on a draft of this paper.

References (45)

  • N.A. Thomas et al.

    Upper and lower visual field differences in perceptual asymmetries

    Brain Research

    (2011)
  • M.H. Van Kleeck

    Hemispheric differences in global versus local processing of hierarchical visual stimuli by normal subjects: New data and a meta-analysis of previous studies

    Neuropsychologia

    (1989)
  • S.E. Asch

    A problem in the theory of associations

    Psychologische Beiträge

    (1962)
  • M.J. Blanca et al.

    Hemispheric differences for global and local processing: Effect of stimulus size and sparsity

    The Spanish Journal of Psychology

    (2009)
  • P. Broca

    Sur le siège de la faculté du langage articulé

    Bulletin de la Société d'Anthropologie

    (1865)
  • K.S. Button et al.

    Power failure: Why small sample size undermines the reliability of neuroscience

    Nature Reviews. Neuroscience

    (2013)
  • S. Christman

    Local-global processing in the upper versus lower visual fields

    Bulletin of the Psychonomic Society

    (1993)
  • D.A. Cook et al.

    Current concepts in validity and reliability for psychometric instruments: theory and application

    The American Journal of Medicine

    (2006)
  • D.C. Delis et al.

    Visuospatial functioning before and after commissurotomy. Disconnection in hierarchical processing

    Archives of Neurology

    (1988)
  • F. Faul et al.

    G*Power 3: A flexible statistical power analysis program for the social, behavioral, and biomedical sciences

    Behavior Research Methods

    (2007)
  • G.R. Fink et al.

    Where in the brain does visual attention select the forest and the trees?

    Nature

    (1996)
  • C. Gerlach

    Normal and abnormal category-effects in visual object recognition: A legacy of Glyn W. Humphreys

    Visual Cognition

    (2017)
  • Cited by (0)

    View full text