Elsevier

Brain Research

Volume 1183, 5 December 2007, Pages 91-108
Brain Research

Research Report
The effects of context, meaning frequency, and associative strength on semantic selection: Distinct contributions from each cerebral hemisphere

https://doi.org/10.1016/j.brainres.2007.09.007Get rights and content

Abstract

The visual half-field procedure was used to examine hemispheric asymmetries in meaning selection. Event-related potentials were recorded as participants decided if a lateralized ambiguous or unambiguous prime was related in meaning to a centrally presented target. Prime–target pairs were preceded by a related or unrelated centrally presented context word. To separate the effects of meaning frequency and associative strength, unambiguous words were paired with concordant weakly related context words and strongly related targets (e.g., taste–sweet–candy) that were similar in associative strength to discordant subordinate-related context words and dominant-related targets (e.g., river–bank–deposit) in the ambiguous condition. Context words and targets were reversed in a second experiment. In an unrelated (neutral) context, N400 responses were more positive than baseline (facilitated) in all ambiguous conditions except when subordinate targets were presented on left visual field–right hemisphere (LVF–RH) trials. Thus, in the absence of biasing context information, the hemispheres seem to be differentially affected by meaning frequency, with the left maintaining multiple meanings and the right selecting the dominant meaning. In the presence of discordant context information, N400 facilitation was absent in both visual fields, indicating that the contextually consistent meaning of the ambiguous word had been selected. In contrast, N400 facilitation occurred in all of the unambiguous conditions; however, the left hemisphere (LH) showed less facilitation for the weakly related target when a strongly related context was presented. These findings indicate that both hemispheres use context to guide meaning selection, but the LH is more likely to focus activation on a single, contextually relevant sense.

Section snippets

Homonymy vs. polysemy

The idea that the RH might activate more broadly and/or be less likely to select a meaning has been tested using both ambiguous homonyms and relatively unambiguous words, but there has been little discussion of the potential differences between these classes of items. Semantic distance in studies using unambiguous items is typically defined in terms of associative strength. This is consistent with the formulation of breadth in the coarse coding hypothesis (Beeman et al., 1994), which argues

Methodological issues

To date, the majority of studies investigating hemispheric asymmetries in the processing of word meanings have relied on reaction time and accuracy data from the lexical decision task, in which participants are asked to decide if a letter string is a word or nonword (for studies using a relatedness judgment task, see Atchley and Kwasny, 2003, Faust and Gernsbacher, 1996, Tompkins et al., 2000, Swaab et al., 1998). Some of the discrepancies among the studies cited above may be due to a lack of

The current study

The current study was designed to further investigate hemispheric asymmetries in meaning processing while taking into consideration the factors just described. In particular, two experiments jointly examine the processing of ambiguous and unambiguous words, using ERPs in conjunction with VF presentation and a relatedness judgment task. Although separate VF studies have examined hemispheric asymmetries in the processing of ambiguous and unambiguous words (e.g., Burgess and Simpson, 1988, Atchley

Recognition accuracy

Mean A′ was .78 (SE = .01)2, indicating that participants attended to the context words and were able to discriminate between these words and distractors.

Relatedness judgment accuracy

Because a delayed response task was utilized, response time data were not analyzed. Accuracy data were subjected to a 2 (Visual Field) × 2 (Ambiguity) × 3 (Relatedness) analysis of variance (ANOVA). None of the interaction effects

Recognition accuracy

Mean A′ was .80 (SE = .01), indicating that participants attended to context words and were able to discriminate between these words and distractors.

Relatedness judgment accuracy

Accuracy data were subjected to a 2 (Visual Field) × 2 (Ambiguity) × 3 (Relatedness) analysis of variance (ANOVA). The main effects of VF [F(1,19) = 16.87, p < .01, MSE = .01], ambiguity [F(1,19) = 28.38, p < .001, MSE = .01], and relatedness [F(2,38) = 32.97, p < .001, MSE = .04] were significant. Similar to Experiment 1, accuracy for LVF–RH trials (M = .78, SE = .02) was

General discussion

The goal of this pair of studies was to examine the ability and tendency of the two cerebral hemispheres to (1) activate strong and weak associates and dominant and subordinate meanings of unambiguous and ambiguous words, respectively, and (2) use context information to shape meaning activation and selection. To that end, ERPs were recorded as participants judged whether or not a lateralized prime word was related in meaning to a centrally presented target word. Prime–target pairs were preceded

Participants

The final set of participants included 20 native English speakers (11 females) with no early (< age 5) exposure to a second language. All participants were right-handed as determined by the Edinburgh handedness inventory (Oldfield, 1971); the mean laterality quotient was 0.78 (range = 0.58 to 1.0), with 1.0 being strongly right-handed and − 1.0 being strongly left-handed. Nine participants reported having immediate family members who were left-handed. The mean age was 19.7 (range 18–25).

Materials

The stimuli

Acknowledgments

We thank Padmapriya Kandhadai for sharing her stimuli, and Sarah Baisley, Alex Harness, Komal Kenkare, Aubrey Lutz, Ed Rice, Emily Selen, and Haq Wajid for assistance with data collection. This research was supported by grant AG026308 to KDF and training grant MH019990 to AMM.

References (71)

  • S.A. Duffy et al.

    Lexical ambiguity and fixation times in reading

    J. Mem. Lang.

    (1988)
  • M. Faust et al.

    Sentence context and lexical ambiguity resolution by the two hemispheres

    Neuropsychologia

    (1998)
  • M.E. Faust et al.

    Cerebral mechanisms for suppression of inappropriate information during sentence comprehension

    Brain Lang.

    (1996)
  • M. Faust et al.

    Sentence priming effects in the two cerebral hemispheres: influences of lexical relatedness, word order, and sentence anomaly

    Neuropsychologia

    (2003)
  • K.D. Federmeier et al.

    Right words and left words: electrophysiological evidence for hemispheric differences in meaning processing

    Cogn. Brain Res.

    (1999)
  • K.D. Federmeier et al.

    Picture the difference: electrophysiological investigations of picture processing in the two cerebral hemispheres

    Neuropsychologia

    (2002)
  • K.D. Federmeier et al.

    Multiple effects of sentential constraint on word processing

    Brain Res.

    (2007)
  • C.M. Grindrod et al.

    Sensitivity to local sentence context information in lexical ambiguity resolution: evidence from left- and right-hemisphere-damaged individuals

    Brain Lang.

    (2003)
  • T.W. Hogaboam et al.

    Lexical ambiguity and sentence comprehension

    J. Verbal Learn. Verbal Behav.

    (1975)
  • P. Holcomb

    Automatic and attentional processing: an event-related brain potential analysis of semantic priming

    Brain Lang.

    (1988)
  • M. Jung-Beeman

    Bilateral brain processes for comprehending natural languages

    Trends Cogn. Sci.

    (2005)
  • P. Kandhadai et al.

    Multiple priming of lexically ambiguous and unambiguous targets in the cerebral hemispheres: the coarse coding hypothesis revisited

    Brain Res.

    (2007)
  • D.E. Klein et al.

    The representation of polysemous words

    J. Mem. Lang.

    (2001)
  • D.E. Klein et al.

    Paper has been my ruin: conceptual relations of polysemous senses

    J. Mem. Lang.

    (2002)
  • E. Klepousniotou et al.

    Processing homonymy and polysemy: effects of sentential context and time-course following unilateral brain damage

    Brain Lang.

    (2005)
  • E. Klepousniotou et al.

    Disambiguating the ambiguity advantage effect in word recognition: an advantage for polysemous but not homonymous words

    J. Neurolinguist.

    (2007)
  • M. Koivisto

    The time course of semantic activation in the cerebral hemispheres

    Neuropsychologia

    (1997)
  • M. Kutas et al.

    Electrophysiology reveals semantic memory use in language comprehension

    Trends Cogn. Sci.

    (2000)
  • R.C. Oldfield

    The assessment and analysis of handedness: the Edinburgh Inventory

    Neuropsychologia

    (1971)
  • J. Rodd et al.

    Making sense of semantic ambiguity: semantic competition in lexical access

    J. Mem. Lang.

    (2002)
  • M.S. Seidenberg et al.

    Automatic access of the meanings of ambiguous words in context: some limitations of knowledge-based processing

    Cogn. Psychol.

    (1982)
  • T.Y. Swaab et al.

    Understanding ambiguous words in sentence contexts: electrophysiological evidence for delayed contextual selection in Broca's aphasia

    Neuropsychologia

    (1998)
  • T. Swaab et al.

    Understanding words in sentence contexts: the time course of ambiguity resolution

    Brain Lang.

    (2003)
  • D.A. Swinney

    Lexical access during sentence comprehension: reconsideration of context effects

    J. Verbal Learn. Verbal Behav.

    (1979)
  • D.A. Swinney et al.

    Effects of prior context upon lexical access during sentence comprehension

    J. Verbal Learn. Verbal Behav.

    (1976)
  • Cited by (28)

    • A prominence-based account of the pragmatics of tenses and the underspecification of the Romance imperfect

      2022, Journal of Pragmatics
      Citation Excerpt :

      In the following, I illustrate the GPH using the different readings expressed by tenses. Several cognitive and psycholinguistic studies have shown that relative frequency plays a role in the accessibility of meanings (e.g., Galbraith and Taschman, 1969; Rubenstein et al., 1970; Becker, 1979; Kawamoto, 1993; Giora, 2003; Meyer and Federmeier, 2007). According to Giora (2003), the higher the frequency of a meaning is in relation to all other available meanings of that entity, the faster the retrieving process of that meaning and, accordingly, the more salient the meaning is.

    • Parallel semantic processing in the flankers task: Evidence from the N400

      2021, Brain and Language
      Citation Excerpt :

      The semantic fit between the word and the preceding sentential context reliably modulates amplitude of the N400, a peak in the ERP waveform that occurs approximately 400 ms after word onset and is associated with lexico-semantic processing (see, e.g., Kutas & Federmeier, 2011, for a review). These modulations of N400 amplitude are graded; for example, expected words generate the smallest negativities, anomalous words generate the largest negativities, and unexpected but plausible words fall somewhere in between (e.g., Kutas & Hillyard, 1983; Meyer & Federmeier, 2007). In the flanker-RSVP paradigm, words are displayed together with the words that occur immediately to the left and right in the sentence.

    View all citing articles on Scopus
    View full text