Elsevier

Behavioural Processes

Volume 142, September 2017, Pages 29-32
Behavioural Processes

Short report
Flexibility in the social behavior of captive female capybaras (Mammalia, Rodentia)

https://doi.org/10.1016/j.beproc.2017.05.018Get rights and content

Highlights

  • Captive female capybaras exhibit a linear social dominance hierarchy.

  • As space per individual decrease there is more predictability in the dominance success.

  • Capybara shows flexibility in social behavior.

Abstract

Capybara (Hydrochoerus hydrochaeris) lives in stable groups composed of adult males and females with their young. The species shows flexibility in social organization in response to short-term environmental changes, but apparently does not show flexibility in social behavior. To gain insights into mechanisms underlying changes in social relationships, we analyzed the social dominance hierarchy of five captive capybara groups, composed of four to 13 adult females kept in outdoor paddocks ranging from 400 to 4500 m2. In addition, we evaluated the effects of group size and space allowance on two complementary properties of social structure: linearity and steepness. Captive female capybaras exhibit a linear social dominance hierarchy. There was also more predictability in the dominance success– hierarchical steepness − in the dominance hierarchy with a decrease in the space per individual. This variability in response to changing circumstances shows flexibility in capybara’s social behavior.

Introduction

The social organization (the composition of groups), the mating system, and the social structure (describing who interacts with whom) concurrently characterize the social system of a species (Kappeler and van Schaik, 2002). The basic unit of capybara (Hydrochoerus hydrochaeris) social organization is the family unit, which consists of a dominant male, several possibly related adult females with their young, and one or more subordinate males (Herrera, 2013). The most common mating system of capybaras is polygynous in which one male is responsible for most of the copulation after pre-copulation competitions (Herrera and Macdonald, 1993). A strictly linear dominance hierarchy characterizes the social structure of capybara males (Herrera and Macdonald, 1993). However, the free-range capybara females do not appear to have a particular social structure among them (Herrera, 2013). In captivity, Ferraz et al. (2013) observed a monopolization of feeding resources by only one female that won conflicts against all other females − characteristic of a despotic dominance hierarchy.

Capybara groups appear to be relatively stable and range from four to 16 adults in the seasonally flooded savannahs of Venezuela (Herrera and Macdonald, 1987). Moreover, the species shows flexibility in social organization −reversible changes in the social organization at a population level in response to short-term changes of the environment (Schradin, 2013). During dry seasons, several groups come together to the dwindling pools, which results in associations of more than 100 capybaras (Ojasti, 1973). In the following rainy season, however, they divide up into their original groups (Herrera and Macdonald, 1987). However, apparently capybara does not show flexibility in social behavior. Flexible social behavior refers to individual variability in response to changing circumstances (Barrett et al., 2013). The increase in capybara group size did not result in an increase in cooperative behavior; on the contrary. (1985) studied the effect of the size of breeding groups (5, 10 or 15 adults) and the space allowance (enclosures with 32 m2 and 120 m2) upon the social dominance hierarchy, agonistic interactions rate, and reproduction. The authors recorded increases in rates of aggression and mortality, as well as a decrease in reproduction rate with the group size. However, there was no relationship between these parameter and space allowance (Ojasti and Sosa Burgos, 1985). Furthermore, Ojasti and Sosa Burgos (1985) verified that low-ranking individuals were the ones that lose weight and died during the study.

The lack of social dominance among free-ranging capybara females (Herrera, 2013) is probably explained because there is no need to compete for grass, the capybara’s main food resource (Ojasti, 1973, Macdonald, 1981), which is dispersed in its range. In contrast, farmed capybaras usually compete for food due to the spatial concentration of this resource. In captivity it is possible to manipulate ecological factors in ways to gain insights into mechanisms underlying changes in social relationships (Calhoun, 1950). Therefore, we aimed to describe the social dominance hierarchy of captive female capybaras and to evaluate the relationship between rank and body mass. We also intended to evaluate the effects of group size and space allowance on two complementary properties of social structure: linearity and steepness. The linearity is the main structural characteristic of a dominance hierarchy that ranges from zero (non-linear hierarchy) to 1.0 (perfect linear hierarchy) (Whitehead, 2009). The hierarchical steepness, a core aspect of social structure, is a continuous measure of dominance gradients measuring predictability of the dominance success (deVries et al., 2006). Adopting a different arrangement of the feeders, to avoid monopolization of feeding resources as occurred in Ferraz et al. (2013), we hypothesized that capybara females organize themselves in linear hierarchy dominance. Following previous results showed by Ojasti and Sosa Burgos (1985), we expected a correlation between rank and body mass independently of the group size and space allowance. We also expected an increase in agonistic rate with the increase of group size independently of the space allowance, as described by Ojasti and Sosa Burgos (1985). Moreover, we predicted decreases in both the hierarchical linearity and steepness along with the increase in group size and decreases in space allowance due to the increase in competition.

Section snippets

Ethical statement

The protocol of this experiment was carried out in accordance with the ‘Guidelines for the use of animals in research’ as published in Animal Behaviour (1991, 41, 183–186).

Study site and animals

The study was carried out with five capybara groups belonging to the Applied Ethology Laboratory, Universidade Estadual de Santa Cruz (UESC), Ilhéus, Brazil. Capybara groups were kept in outdoor paddocks ranging from 450 to 4500 m2, surrounded by a chain-link fence, with earth underfoot, low bushes, an artificial tank made of

Results and discussion

During feeding time we observed at least three agonistic interactions between all possible dyads in all but the smallest group. Females’ hierarchy linearity index ranged from h = 0.73 (P = 0.0001), in the largest group, to h = 1.0 (P = 0.03) in the groups with five and six females (Table 2). In the group with four females the h’ index showed a strictly linear hierarchy, but without significance (P = 0.37, Table 2), probably due to the few interactions directed and received by the two most submissive

Conclusions

Our results showed that captive female capybaras exhibit a linear social dominance hierarchy. We also found more predictability in the dominance success– hierarchical steepness − in the dominance hierarchy in capybara groups with less space allowance per individual. This variability in response to changing circumstances shows flexibility in capybara’s social behavior.

Acknowledgments

We thank Dr. Adriana Maldonado-Chaparro and Michele Singh for providing helpful comments on an earlier draft of this manuscript. We also thank the Associate Editor Patrica Izar and the three anonymous reviewers of Behavioural Processes for their valuable comments on the final manuscript. SSCN and SLGNF were supported by the National Council of Technological and Scientific Development (CNPq Process # 303743/2013-1 and 303589/2015-9, respectively). We are also grateful to all the staff of the

References (21)

There are more references available in the full text version of this article.

Cited by (1)

  • Prosociality and reciprocity in capybaras (Hydrochoerus hydrochaeris) in a non-reproductive context

    2021, Behavioural Processes
    Citation Excerpt :

    However, because our experiment was conducted in a non-reproductive context, we did not assume that females would be widely more prosocial than males. A strict linear social dominance hierarchy enforced by chasing and, rarely, fights has been described in male capybaras (Herrera and Macdonald, 1989, 1993; Maldonado-Chaparro and Blumstein, 2008), and more recently in female capybaras (Nogueira-Filho et al., 2017). Considering the stable and linear hierarchy within our subjects (both males and females), subordinates might be more prosocial towards dominants to avoid punishment.

View full text