Elsevier

Behavioural Brain Research

Volume 177, Issue 1, 12 February 2007, Pages 51-60
Behavioural Brain Research

Research report
Enhancing effects of chronic lithium on memory in the rat

https://doi.org/10.1016/j.bbr.2006.11.003Get rights and content

Abstract

Background

In spite of recent enrichment of neurochemical and behavioural data establishing a neuroprotective role for lithium, its primary effects on cognitive functioning remain ambiguous. This study examines chronic lithium effects on spatial working memory and long-term retention.

Methods

In three discrete experiments, rats subjected to 30 daily intraperitoneal injections (2 mmol/kg) of lithium (lithium groups: serum lithium = 0.5 ± 0.4 mEq/l, 12 h post-injection) or saline (controls) were trained in 0-s delay T-maze alternation and then tested in 30-, 45- and 60-s delay alternation (Experiments 1, 2, 3, respectively). Animals from Experiment 1 were further tested in one-trial step-through passive avoidance under mild shock parameters (0.5 mA, 1 s). Retention was assessed 6 h later. Daily lithium or saline injections continued throughout behavioural testing.

Results

Lithium animals were indistinguishable from controls during 0-delay alternation baseline (Experiments 1–3, accuracy > 88%) but showed significantly higher accuracy than controls at 30- and 45-s delays (93% versus 85% and 92% versus 82%, Experiments 1 and 2, respectively). At 60-s delay (Experiment 3) this beneficial effect of lithium was no longer apparent (lithium and control accuracy = 78%). In Experiment 4, the shock used did not support 6-h passive avoidance retention in controls, whereas lithium animals showed significant step-through latency increases.

Conclusions

Chronic lithium enhanced spatial working memory and promoted long-term retention of a weak aversive contingency. The results suggest that lithium may have potential as a cognitive enhancer.

Introduction

The mood-stabilizing agent lithium is the drug of choice in the treatment of and prophylaxis against both mania and depression in bipolar disorder [1], [2], [3], [4], [5]. In addition to its established role as a mood stabiliser, a plethora of recent findings attribute a neuroprotective [6], [7], [8], [9], [10], [11] and an antiapoptotic [12], [13] role to lithium.

The neuroprotective effect of lithium, which has been associated with long-term administration of therapeutic levels of the substance [9] raises expectations regarding its potential as a prophylactic agent against cognitive decline. However, early clinical reports linked lithium treatment to cognitive blurring and memory deficits (for review see [14]). More recent neuropsychological testing has yielded ambiguous results, associating lithium treatment with subjective complaints but not with actual impairments in terms of performance accuracy [15]. There have been several studies which tested the effects of lithium treatment on different types of memory, in psychiatric (mainly bipolar) patients [16], [17], [18], [19], [20], [21], in bipolar patients compared to healthy volunteers [22], [23], [24], [25] and in normal subjects [26], [27], [28], [29]. The latter studies have the advantage of isolating the neuropsychological effects of lithium from those of bipolar disease processes.

In terms of lithium effects on short-term verbal memory performance, several studies report that lithium treatment caused deficits in immediate recall [16], [17], [18], [20], [22], [23], [24], [25], [28], [29] while only three studies failed to replicate this finding [19], [21], [26].

In most studies investigating lithium effects on long-term verbal memory lithium was found to have a negative impact on delayed recall tasks [16], [17], [20], [23], [24], [25], [28]. Nevertheless, according to Pachet and Wisniewski [15] who reviewed these studies, the trend toward verbal memory impairment in the lithium-treated population was relatively weak and replications are needed to confirm it.

In contrast to its reported effects on verbal memory, no significant effects of lithium emerge in the area of visual memory. The immediate form of visual memory tasks was reported unimpaired by lithium administration in most relevant studies [18], [21], [23], [25]. A single study [16] reported transient impairments in visual memory which dissipated following discontinuation of lithium. With respect to delayed recall in visual memory tasks, one study [18] reported no lithium-associated decrease in performance, whereas a later study [16] found reversible lithium-induced deficits.

The animal literature on the cognitive effects of lithium presents similar inconsistencies. Older studies suggest that lithium induces cognitive deficits, which they attribute to a narrowing of attentional filtering onto high salience stimuli. Hines and Poling [30] reported hindered acquisition of passive avoidance with no effect on active avoidance after lithium pretreatment. Hines [31], [32] observed deficits in the acquisition of position discrimination and compromised shock-induced activity suppression in the open field. Cappeliez and Moore [33] and Cappeliez et al. [34] reported increased attention to high salience cues and impaired latent inhibition in lithium-treated rats.

Recent animal studies on the effects of lithium in various memory tasks are sparse. A transient deficit in spatial reference but not working memory was observed in the hole board task after chronic lithium [35]. The authors attributed this deficit to the suppressive effect of lithium on the basal expression of the immediate-early gene Nurr1 (implicated in neuronal plasticity) in the hippocampus. However, Vasconcellos et al. [36], investigating the effects of chronic lithium treatment on reference and working memory in a chronic stress model, reported no effect of lithium on reference memory in the water maze. Furthermore, they showed that the reference memory deficit induced by chronic stress was attenuated by lithium treatment. In the same study, neither stress nor lithium had any effect on working memory. The only recent study suggesting that chronic lithium impairs spatial working and reference memory used spontaneous alternation in a plus maze in black molly fish [37].

In summary, recent human studies suggest that chronic lithium causes subtle negative effects on psychomotor speed and verbal memory but no impairment on visuospatial skills, attention or concentration [15]. Animal studies suggest some behavioural deficits in active avoidance and visually cued discrimination learning, while spatial learning and memory appear to be transiently if at all impaired, or even protected by lithium pretreatment [36]. These recent findings are more in line than older studies with the biochemical literature which establishes a neuroprotective role for chronic lithium treatment. They are also in line with some early findings of our laboratory, which suggested a beneficial effect of chronic lithium on working memory in the rat [38], thus raising the possibility that lithium may act as a cognitive enhancer. This hypothesis was further investigated in the series of experiments reported here. These experiments assessed the effects of chronic lithium, at doses sustaining lithium plasma levels within the human clinical range, on spatial working memory and long-term retention.

Section snippets

Materials and methods

Experiments 1 and 4 were carried out in the Experimental Psychology laboratory, Institute of Psychiatry, University of London [38] while Experiments 2 and 3 were conducted in the Experimental Psychology laboratory, Department of Psychiatry, University of Athens Medical School. For this reason Experiments 1–3, all of which involve delayed alternation differing in delay length only (30-, 45- and 60-s, respectively) were carried out as discrete procedures rather than as a repeated-measures

Serum lithium levels following acute or chronic LiCl treatment

Serum lithium levels sustained by a daily dose of 2 mmol/kg i.p. are shown in Fig. 1. There was no evidence of lithium accumulation after chronic treatment, as indicated by the lack of difference in serum lithium levels produced by a single LiCl injection preceded either by 29 saline or by 29 LiCl daily injections (factor of pre-treatment length = NS). Plasma levels did change significantly over post-injection time (factor of post-injection time: F(2, 18) = 34.15, p < 0.001). The dose of 2 mmol/kg

Discussion

This study addressed the controversy emerging from human and animal studies with respect to the effects of lithium on cognitive functioning, memory in particular. We examined the effect of chronic lithium on basic memory processes, taking special care to use doses which would sustain serum lithium levels within the clinical therapeutic window and would not lead to tissue accumulation of the substance after prolonged administration, with possible detriment to animals’ general state of health. As

Acknowledgements

We would like to thank Dr. Chrysoula Nikolaou and the Microbiology laboratory of Eginition Hospital for the serum Li+ assays. We are also grateful to Dr. Jeffrey W. Dalley for his comments on the manuscript.

References (53)

  • T.K. Creson et al.

    Dose–response effects of chronic lithium regimens on spatial memory in the black molly fish

    Pharmacol Biochem Behav

    (2003)
  • B. Givens et al.

    Bidirectional modulation of scopolamine-induced working memory impairments by muscarinic activation of the medial septal area

    Neurobiol Learn Mem

    (1995)
  • J.N. Rawlins et al.

    The septo-hippocampal system and cognitive mapping

    Behav Brain Res

    (1982)
  • F.A. De Souza et al.

    Intracerebroventricular effects of angiotensin II on a stepthrough passive avoidance task in rats

    Neurobiol Learn Mem

    (2004)
  • T. Masaki et al.

    Taste aversion in rats induced by forced swimming, voluntary running, forced running, and lithium chloride injection treatments

    Physiol Behav

    (2006)
  • M. Nachman et al.

    Learned taste aversions in rats as a function of dosage, concentration, and route of administration of LiCl

    Physiol Behav

    (1973)
  • H. Prior et al.

    Dissociation of spatial reference memory, spatial working memory and hippocampal mossy fiber distribution in two rat strains differing in emotionality

    Behav Brain Res

    (1997)
  • T. Pascual et al.

    A protective effect of lithium on rat behaviour altered by ibotenic acid lesions of the basal forebrain cholinergic system.

    Brain Res

    (1995)
  • S.M. Carney et al.

    Lithium—a continuing story in the treatment of bipolar disorder

    Acta Psychiatr Scand

    (2005)
  • D.J. Muzina et al.

    Maintenance therapies in bipolar disorder: focus on randomized controlled trials

    Aust NZ J Psychiatry

    (2005)
  • M.E. Thase

    Bipolar depression: issues in diagnosis and treatment

    Harv Rev Psychiatry

    (2005)
  • A.H. Young et al.

    Lithium in maintenance therapy for bipolar disorder

    J Psychopharmacol

    (2006)
  • G. Chen et al.

    The mood stabilizing agents lithium and valproate robustly increase the levels of the neuroprotective protein bcl-2 in the CNS

    J Neurochem

    (1999)
  • D.M. Chuang et al.

    Neuroprotective effects of lithium in cultured cells and animal models of diseases

    Bipolar Disord

    (2002)
  • D.M. Chuang

    Neuroprotective and neurotrophic actions of the mood stabilizer lithium: can it be used to treat neurodegenerative diseases?

    Crit Rev Neurobiol

    (2004)
  • D.M. Chuang

    The antiapoptotic actions of mood stabilizers: molecular mechanisms and therapeutic potentials

    Ann NY Acad Sci

    (2005)
  • Cited by (0)

    1

    Tel.: +30 210 7289114.

    2

    Tel.: +44 1223 765290.

    3

    Tel.: +30 210 7289219.

    4

    Tel.: +30 210 7289410.

    View full text