Elsevier

Archives of Gerontology and Geriatrics

Volume 78, September–October 2018, Pages 139-149
Archives of Gerontology and Geriatrics

Cardiovascular symptoms and longitudinal declines in processing speed differentially predict cerebral white matter lesions in older adults

https://doi.org/10.1016/j.archger.2018.06.010Get rights and content

Highlights

  • We used a data mining approach to compare 52 predictors of white matter lesion burden.

  • Strongest predictors were age, cardiovascular symptoms, and processing speed decline.

  • WML-cognition relations may be etiologically heterogeneous across cerebral regions.

Abstract

It is well established that cerebral white matter lesions (WML), present in the majority of older adults, are associated with cardiovascular and cerebrovascular diseases and also with cognitive decline. However, much less is known about how WML are related to other important individual characteristics and about the generality vs. brain region-specificity of WML. In a longitudinal study of 112 community-dwelling adults (age 50–71 years at study entry), we used a machine learning approach to evaluate the relative strength of 52 variables in association with WML burden. Variables included socio-demographic, lifestyle, and health indices—as well as multiple cognitive abilities (modeled as latent constructs using factor analysis)—repeatedly measured at three- to six-year intervals. Greater chronological age, symptoms of cardiovascular disease, and processing speed declines were most strongly linked to elevated WML burden (accounting for ∼49% of variability in WML). Whereas frontal lobe WML burden was associated both with elevated cardiovascular symptoms and declines in processing speed, temporal lobe WML burden was only significantly associated with declines in processing speed. These latter outcomes suggest that age-related WML-cognition associations may be etiologically heterogeneous across fronto-temporal cerebral regions.

Introduction

Cerebral white matter lesions (WML) are present in the majority of older adults and appear to play a key role in cognitive decrements, stroke, dementia, and increased mortality risk (Debette & Markus, 2010). These can be quantified as hyperintensities on Fluid Attenuated Inversion Recovery (FLAIR) magnetic resonance images (MRI). Ample evidence from several large-sample studies of community-dwelling adults implicates cardiovascular and cerebrovascular diseases as key factors in the development and progression of WML in later life, and frontal/anterior regions of the brain appear most affected at initial onset (Kennedy & Raz, 2009). With respect to changes in specific cognitive abilities, WML burden most consistently predicts declines in processing speed and executive function (Gunning-Dixon, Brickman, Cheng, & Alexopoulos, 2009; Jagust, 2013; Kloppenborg, Nederkoorn, Geerlings, & van den Berg, 2014). Thus, for example, there appears to be general consensus that cerebral small-vessel ischemia leads to WM degradation (e.g., demyelination, rarefication) within association fiber tracts and, by extension, to impaired functional connectivity across cortical networks. This process may play a key role in age-related cognitive deficits in older adults (Kennedy & Raz, 2009; Madden et al., 2012; Prins & Scheltens, 2015).

However, associations between WML burden and age-related cognitive decrements have been shown to persist even after statistical adjustment for the presence of vascular disorders and associated risk factors, such as smoking (Jagust, 2013; Kloppenborg et al., 2014). This suggests that other variables may act independently of, or interact with, vascular diseases to influence WML-cognition relations. For example, sleep duration has been shown to moderate the relation between diabetes mellitus and WML burden (Ramos et al., 2014), and depression has been implicated in both direct and indirect relations with WM degradation (Madden et al., 2012; Malloy, Correia, Stebbins, & Laidlaw, 2007). These variables may reflect specific neurochemical imbalances. Genetic predisposition may also play a role: Individuals with the E4 allele of Apolipoprotein E (APOE), a gene involved in the regulation of cholesterol transport, may be at greater risk for developing WML (Cherbuin, Leach, Christensen, & Anstey, 2007; Paternoster, Chen, & Sudlow, 2009; Raz, Yang, Dahle, & Land, 2012). Additionally, higher education and socio-economic status during youth have been linked to reduced WML prevalence in later life (Murray, McNeil, Salarirad, Whalley, & Staff, 2014), but education may also attenuate or mask the effects WM degradation on age-sensitive cognitive declines (Brickman et al., 2011; Christensen et al., 2007).

As for specific cognitive outcomes, WML-related decrements in lower-order abilities, such as processing speed, may mediate decrements in higher-order abilities, such as fluid intelligence and abstract problem-solving (Kievit et al., 2016). There is also evidence that WM degradation across brain regions is differentially associated with decrements in processing speed and in fluid abilities. For example, Marquine et al. (2010) found that total and anterior WML progression was associated with processing speed decrements, whereas posterior WML progression was linked to declines in visuo-constructional ability. More recently, MRI diffusion tensor imaging (DTI) studies have shown that WM diffusivity in select cerebral regions accounts for variability in fluid intelligence not accounted for by processing speed or age (Borghesani et al., 2013) and that multiple WM tracts are differentially associated with decrements in processing speed and in fluid intelligence (Gazes et al., 2016). However, it remains unclear whether WM deterioration mediates associations between decrements in higher order abilities, such as abstract reasoning, and declines in processing speed (Madden, Bennett, & Song, 2009).

Regional specificity in brain-cognition associations has also been observed in research using volumetric and cortical thickness measures of brain atrophy (e.g., Fjell et al., 2006; Golomb et al., 1994; Raz, Briggs, Marks, & Acker, 1999; Schretlen et al., 2000; Tisserand, Visser, van Boxtel, & Jolles, 2000). Raz et al. (2008) observed that brain volume losses across prefrontal and medio-temporal regions were differentially associated with vascular risk and fluid intelligence deficits, results they interpreted as dissociation between prefrontal and medial-temporal systems. Others have similarly suggested that different etiological profiles may underpin neurodegeneration in frontal vs. temporal regions, e.g., due to “normal” aging vs. pathological aging (Fjell, McEvoy, Holland, Dale, & Walhovd, 2014). Such differences would likely be reflected in different profiles of cognitive decline (e.g., decrements in processing speed vs. memory). Additionally, although there is some evidence to suggest that loss of brain volume may account for relations between WML progression and cognitive declines (Schmidt et al., 2005), results from other studies indicate that brain atrophy does not influence WML-cognitive associations (Borghesani et al., 2013; Marquine et al., 2010; Silbert, Nelson, Howieson, Moore, & Kaye, 2008; van den Heuvel et al., 2006).

At present, knowledge of WML-cognition relations mostly comes from cross-sectional (single time point) studies wherein WML was assessed globally and in which cognitive measures were modeled as singular outcomes (Marquine et al., 2010). Covariates, if included, have often been limited to age, sex, and educational status (Kloppenborg et al., 2014). But in a recent large-sample cohort study (with assessments at ages 73 and 76 years), Ritchie et al. (2015) examined level-change associations between multiple cognitive abilities (modeled as latent constructs) and cerebral WM volumes (both for normal appearing tissue and lesion prevalence). Changes in all cognitive abilities were negatively associated with changes in WM lesion prevalence. With respect to level-change relations, better baseline performance in all cognitive domains predicted less decline in normal appearing white matter volume, but only better baseline performance in processing speed predicted less increase in WM lesion prevalence. More recently, Ritchie et al. (2017) found that an aggregate measure of intelligence (mainly verbal ability) was modestly negatively associated with baseline level of normal appearing WM volume, but not WM lesion prevalence, when statistically adjusted for differences in eleven socio-demographic, health-related, and genetic covariates. In both of these studies, cognitive predictors were not mutually-conditioned, so the relative contribution of each cognitive domain for predicting WM health is difficult to gauge. Notwithstanding, the results suggest that specific cognitive abilities, both at baseline and longitudinally, may be differentially implicated in WM associations.

In brief, current empirical evidence most consistently frames cerebral WML burden as an intermediating condition linking vascular diseases to decrements in processing speed and executive ability. However, evidence of differential associations between distinct cognitive abilities (and levels and changes thereof) and region-specific WML burden suggests a broader scope for etiologies implicated in WML-cognition associations. These associations are likely complex, and a comprehensive analytical approach in which multiple cognitive abilities are examined concurrently with other longitudinal covariates may serve to further clarify the importance of different variables linked to WML burden.

Toward this end, in a sample of 112 older adults, we evaluated the relative strength of association of 52 variables—including socio-demographic indices, longitudinal measures of lifestyle and health, and longitudinal measures of cognitive performance (processing speed, crystallized intelligence, and fluid intelligence)—with respect to regional and total cerebral WML. We applied two different statistical methodologies for this purpose. First, we used random forest analysis (RFA), a machine learning approach, to evaluate all 52 variables as concurrently related to WML burden. RFA is an extension of regression trees, a non-parametric statistical method in which observations are recursively partitioned to identify variables most strongly associated with the outcome of interest (Aichele, Rabbitt, & Ghisletta, 2016; Breiman, 2001; Strobl, Malley, & Tutz, 2009). Unlike more common statistical approaches based on regression, RFA implicitly adjusts for all possible linear, non-linear, and higher-order interaction effects. It also provides built-in safeguards against multicollinearity and model over-fit. However, because RFA was not developed within a standard probabilistic framework, we also examined subsets of the most important predictors (as determined by RFA) using generalized linear regression (GLR), which allowed us to estimate effect sizes for the most important variables associated with WML.

In light of previous research findings, we anticipated that chronological age and symptoms of cardiovascular disease would weigh strongest in associations with WML (both within and across cerebral lobes). Additionally, we expected that processing speed decrements would be closely associated with elevated WML even after accounting for age and cardiovascular risk—and that this association would appear strongest for total WML and for frontal lobe WML. We further expected to find some evidence for an independent association between fluid intelligence and WML after accounting for differences in symptoms of cardiovascular illnesses and processing speed decrements.

Section snippets

Participants

Data for these analyses were obtained from 65 women and 47 men who were participants in the Manchester Longitudinal Study of Cognition (MLSC; see Rabbitt et al., 2004 for details). The project was approved by the Department of Psychology Internal Ethics Committee, University of Manchester; by the University of Manchester Ethics Committee, and by the Greater Manchester NIH Trust Ethics Committee. Participants’ ages ranged 50–71 years (M = 60.9) at study inception and 62–86 years (M = 73.8) at

Results

Outcomes from the random forest analysis and from the generalized linear regression analysis are presented in Table 4. Only variables included in the GLR analysis (Irel ≥ .25) are shown. Thus, regression models included between three and seven predictors. We conducted a post-hoc power analysis to approximate the probability of detecting weak, moderate, and strong effects given these different numbers of predictors as based on criteria suggested by Cohen (1992). Power to detect weak effects

Discussion

In a longitudinal study of community-dwelling middle-aged and older adults, we examined the relative strength of associations of cognitive, socio-demographic, lifestyle, and health variables with WML burden. Of these variables, we found that higher chronological age at time of MRI scan, more symptoms of cardiovascular disease, and sharper declines in processing speed were most strongly (and consistently) associated with increased WML burden—jointly accounting for approximately 49% of

Conclusions

To date, few studies have examined longitudinal measures of multiple cognitive abilities as concurrently associated with WML burden in community-dwelling older adults. And we know of no other study that has accounted for such a large range of socio-demographic, lifestyle, and health-related covariates (or that has used a sophisticated machine learning methodology) to account for these associations. In general, chronological age, symptoms of cardiovascular illness (especially hypertension), and

Funding

The authors report no conflicting financial interests pertaining to this research.

Acknowledgments

This work was supported by the Swiss National Science Foundation (grant number 100019_146535), by the Swiss National Centre of Competence in Research LIVES – Overcoming vulnerability: Life course perspectives (which is financed by the Swiss National Science Foundation, grant number: 51NF40-160590), the UK Medical Research Council, the UK Economic and Social Research Council, and the UK Wellcome Trust. The authors are grateful to these organizations for their financial assistance. We also thank

References (85)

  • N. Pendleton et al.

    Concordance of Cornell medical index self-reports to structured clinical assessment for the identification of physical health status

    Archives of Gerontology and Geriatrics

    (2004)
  • N. Raz et al.

    Volume of white matter hyperintensities in healthy adults: Contribution of age, vascular risk factors, and inflammation-related genetic variants

    Biochimica et Biophysica Acta

    (2012)
  • P. Scheltens et al.

    A semiquantitative rating scale for the assessment of signal hyperintensities on magnetic resonance imaging

    Journal of the Neurological Sciences

    (1993)
  • D.J. Tisserand et al.

    The relation between global and limbic brain volumes on MRI and cognitive performance in healthy individuals across the age range

    Neurobiology of Aging

    (2000)
  • L.R. Williams et al.

    Clinical correlates of cerebral white matter hyperintensities in cognitively normal older adults

    Archives of Gerontology and Geriatrics

    (2010)
  • J.A. Yesavage et al.

    Development and validation of a geriatric depression screening scale: A preliminary report

    Journal of Psychiatric Research

    (1983)
  • S. Aichele et al.

    Lifespan decrements in fluid intelligence and processing speed predict mortality risk

    Psychology and Aging

    (2015)
  • S. Aichele et al.

    Think fast, feel fine, live long: A 29-year study of cognition, health, and survival in middle-aged and older adults

    Psychological Science

    (2016)
  • P.D. Allison

    Logistic regression using SAS theory and application

    (2012)
  • A.D. Baddeley et al.

    The speech and capacity of language processing (SCOLP) test

    (1992)
  • M. Boukobza et al.

    Sturge-Weber syndrome. The current neuroradiologic data

    Journal de Radiologie

    (2000)
  • L. Breiman

    Random forests

    Machine Learning

    (2001)
  • K. Brodman et al.

    Manual for the cornell medical index, health questionnaire

    (1949)
  • A.C. Cameron et al.

    R-squared measures for count data regression models with applications to health-care utilization

    Journal of Business and Economic Statistics

    (1996)
  • R.B. Cattell et al.

    Champaign, IL: IPAT

    The individual or group culture fair intelligence test

    (1960)
  • N. Cherbuin et al.

    Neuroimaging and APOE genotype: A systematic qualitative review

    Dementia and Geriatric Cognitive Disorders

    (2007)
  • H. Christensen et al.

    The brain reserve hypothesis, brain atrophy and aging

    Gerontology

    (2007)
  • J. Cohen

    A power primer

    Psychological Bulletin

    (1992)
  • F.-E. de Leeuw et al.

    Prevalence of cerebral white matter lesions in elderly people: A population based magnetic resonance imaging study. The Rotterdam scan study

    Journal of Neurology, Neurosurgery, & Psychiatry

    (2001)
  • S. Debette et al.

    The clinical importance of white matter hyperintensities on brain magnetic resonance imaging: Systematic review and meta-analysis

    BMJ

    (2010)
  • S. Debette et al.

    Midlife vascular risk factor exposure accelerates structural brain aging and cognitive decline

    Neurology

    (2011)
  • P.J. Diggle et al.

    Analysis of longitudinal data

    (2002)
  • F.R. Ferraro et al.

    Preliminary normative data on the geriatric depression scale-short form (GDS-SF) in a young adult sample

    Journal of Clinical Psychology

    (1996)
  • P. Ghisletta et al.

    Age-based structural dynamics between perceptual speed and knowledge in the Berlin aging study: Direct evidence for ability dedifferentiation in old age

    Psychology and Aging

    (2003)
  • J. Golomb et al.

    Hippocampal formation size in normal human aging: A correlate of delayed secondary memory performance

    Learning & Memory

    (1994)
  • F.M. Gunning-Dixon et al.

    The cognitive correlates of white matter abnormalities in normal aging: A quantitative review

    Neuropsychology

    (2000)
  • F.M. Gunning-Dixon et al.

    Aging of cerebral white matter: A review of MRI findings

    Journal of Geriatric Psychiatry

    (2009)
  • A.W. Heim

    AH4 Group Test of General Intelligence

    (1970)
  • M.d.C. Hernández et al.

    New multispectral MRI data fusion technique for white matter lesion segmentation: Method and comparison with thresholding in FLAIR images

    European Radiology

    (2010)
  • T. Hothorn et al.

    Survival ensembles

    Biostatistics

    (2006)
  • R.P. Kloppenborg et al.

    Presence and progression of white matter hyperintensities and cognition (a meta-analysis)

    Neurology

    (2014)
  • N.M. Laird et al.

    Random-effects models for longitudinal data

    Biometrics

    (1982)
  • Cited by (0)

    View full text