Elsevier

Food Quality and Preference

Volume 10, Issue 6, November 1999, Pages 457-464
Food Quality and Preference

The consumption of convenience foods: reference groups and eating situations

https://doi.org/10.1016/S0950-3293(99)00042-7Get rights and content

Abstract

The theory of reasoned action was applied to study situational influence on the consumption of TV dinners. We investigated five situations, which were either time-related (weekdays vs weekends) or social (“dinner alone”, “dinner with family”, and “dinner with friends”). The intention to use a TV dinner decreased from “alone” via “with family” to “with friends”, but did not differ between weekdays and weekends. Subjective norms were a stronger influence on intentions than attitudes in all situations, except for “weekdays” and “dinner alone”. Primary reference groups were a stronger influence on intentions than secondary reference groups, and the motivation to comply with a particular reference group increased substantially when it joined for dinner. Consumption frequency for TV dinners was higher in households where the person responsible for meal preparation held a paid job, and it was positively related to the number of hours that this person was employed.

Introduction

The role of convenience in food choice and preference has been described in several studies. It has been shown that perceived convenience is strongly correlated with preferences for meals and snacks (Rappoport, Peters, Downey, McCann & Huff-Corzine, 1993), and with preferences for different packages for beverages (van Dam & van Trijp, 1994). Also, Anderson and Shugan (1991) found that the increasing demand for chicken meat is partly due to the increasing appreciation of the fact that chicken is easier to prepare than beef. The importance of convenience reflects a tendency to minimise the time and effort that is put into the preparation of meals. Various strategies may be followed to accomplish this. Examples include eating convenience foods, buying take-away meals, eating in (fast food) restaurants and using convenient appliances such as microwaves (cf. Bryant, 1988, Darian, and Tucci, 1992, Kim, 1989, Oropesa, 1993, Reilly, 1982). These strategies, however, will not be applied in all situations, and different strategies are applied in different situations (Darian, and Tucci, 1992, Nickols and Fox, 1983).

Research on food choice and preference has paid little attention to situational influence (Meiselman, 1996). The most important studies are those of Lahteenmaki, and Tuorila, 1998, Miller and Ginter, 1979, Termorshuizen, Meulenberg, and Wierenga, 1986. Miller and Ginter found that eating situations had a strong impact on the choice for fast-food restaurants. Termorshuizen et al. found that the attitude toward a beverage could vary considerably between situations (i.e. breakfast, lunch, afternoon and evening). For example, attitudes toward coffee were highest for evening and lowest for breakfast, while the reverse was true for fruit yoghurt drinks. Lahteenmaki and Tuorila found similar effects for the intention to consume milk drinks and fruit juice.

We seek to expand extant knowledge on situational influence in the consumption of convenience foods. Specifically, we will study the role of eating situations in the consumption of TV dinners. TV dinners are meals bought in stores, and prepared at home by re-heating. The consumption of such meals reduces the time and effort necessary for meal preparation. As such, TV dinners are a prototypical convenience food. Because the use of TV dinners seems to be more appropriate in some situations than in others, they provide an interesting subject for studying situational influence.

Consumer researchers have found that consumer behaviour is better understood when situational influences are taken into consideration (cf. Assael, 1995). Predictions of consumer choice through first and second order Markov models have been shown to improve significantly when situational influences are accounted for (Umesh & Cote, 1988). Miller and Ginter (1979) found that the performance of multi-attribute models improves when a situational variable is incorporated. Like Lahteenmaki and Tuorila (1998), Miller and Ginter found that behavioural intentions vary across situations. In addition, they found that the evaluations of several attributes of fast-food restaurants (i.e. “speed of service”, “convenience”, and “variety of menu”) differed significantly between situations. Similar results were reported by Termorshuizen et al. (1986) for their study on beverages. We now can formulate the following two hypotheses:

H1a

Consumers' intention to consume TV dinners varies significantly between eating situations.

H1b

The evaluations of (at least some) attributes of TV dinners differ significantly between eating situations.

In line with Miller and Ginter (1979), and Lahteenmaki and Tuorila (1998), we investigate situational influence in an experiment-like setting, in which situational variables are manipulated within subjects. Specifically, we ask respondents to answer questions about their preference for convenience foods in different situations. The explicit measurement of preferences in clearly defined situations avoids confounding effects of situational variables. To study the role of situations in the consumption of convenience foods in more detail, we focus on two aspects of particular interest, namely, time and social surroundings. Below, we will discuss these factors, and their relevance for convenience food consumption.

Convenience foods are “...fully or partially prepared foods in which significant preparation time, culinary skills, or energy inputs have been transferred from the home kitchen to the food processor and distributor” (Capps, Tedford & Havlicek, 1985). According to this definition, time saving is an important characteristic for convenience foods, which has been acknowledged by several studies on the effect of time pressure on the consumption of convenience foods (cf. Darian & Tucci, 1992). In these studies, time pressure is measured by the working status of the person who is responsible for meal preparation. The results of these studies have been unequivocal. The use of convenient appliances like dishwashers and microwaves is higher when the person who is responsible for meal preparation holds a job. The same finding has been reported for the consumption of meals away from home, but not for the consumption of convenience foods (Bryant, 1988, Darian, and Tucci, 1992, Kim, 1989, Yen, 1993). Darian and Tucci attribute these results to the fact that meals away from home are a suitable alternative for cooking, while convenience foods are not. Previous research on the relation between convenience food consumption and working status was done in the US. The study of this relation in a different cultural setting, namely, the Netherlands, provides an opportunity to examine whether these findings generalise across cultures. Regarding the relation between working status and the consumption of convenience foods, we hypothesise that:

H2a

The consumption frequency for TV dinners is higher for households in which the person who is responsible for meal preparation has a paid job.

H2b

The consumption frequency for TV dinners increases with the number of hours of paid employment of the person who is responsible for meal preparation.

It is widely recognised that consumer behaviour is influenced by social surroundings (Bearden, and Etzel, 1991, Belk, 1975). For food products, however, it is often argued that the role of social influence is negligible, and that behaviour is primarily determined by individual preferences and nutritional needs (Bearden & Etzel, 1991). Some authors have opposed this view. Furst, Connors, Bisogni, Sobal and Winter Falk (1996), for example, found that the management of (family) relationships is an important influence on food choice. Pliner and Chaiken (1990) and Rozin (1996) point out that food choice is strongly dependent on the social and cultural context of the choice situation. These authors argue that food preferences vary widely between cultures and social classes, and that the acceptability of food items is shaped by education, upbringing, advertisements, press reports and word-of-mouth. A more immediate form of social influence is provided by the direct social context of eating. Meals are often eaten in the presence of others, and the acceptability of alternatives depends on the company for which the meal is prepared (cf. Furst et al., 1996). Obviously, the social influence on the intention to consume a TV dinner when a consumer is alone, differs widely from the social influence on this intention when the consumer has a family dinner or a cosy meal with friends. We hence hypothesise that:

H3a

Social setting has a strong impact on the social influence on consumers' behavioural intentions regarding the consumption of TV dinners. In particular, social influence is strongest when others are part of the eating situation.

Social influence is performed by reference groups, consisting of one or more persons or institutions. Reference groups can be classified as primary or secondary. Primary reference groups are characterised by strong (emotional) ties and frequent interpersonal interactions. This category generally includes partner and children. Secondary reference groups may include friends, colleagues, and institutions like government and media. Emotional ties with these groups are weaker and interactions are less frequent. In general, primary reference groups are more likely to influence behaviour than secondary reference groups (Assael, 1995). However, as we have mentioned earlier, the influence of a particular reference group depends in part on the social context of the meal. When friends or family join for dinner, for example, the person who is preparing the meal will be more likely to comply with their preferences and opinions.

H3b

When social surroundings are not specified, primary reference groups exert a stronger influence on the consumption of TV dinners than secondary reference groups do.

H3c

The extent to which a particular reference group influences behaviour is affected by the social surroundings of the behaviour. In particular, the influence of a reference group increases when this group (or person) joins for dinner.

We studied the influence of situational variables within the well-known framework of the theory of reasoned action (TRA, Fishbein & Ajzen, 1975). TRA is widely adopted by social scientists, and provides a reasonably adequate forecast of consumer behaviour and intentions (cf. Ajzen and Fishbein, 1980, Shepherd, 1990). TRA is based on the assumption that an individual's behaviour is largely determined by his or her behavioural intentions. These intentions (BIact), are determined by the person's attitude towards the behaviour (Aact), and subjective norms (SNact):BIact1Aact2SNact

To predict buying behaviour, a consumer's attitude towards buying a product is inferred from the summed expectations with regards to n relevant product attributes. These expectations are obtained by multiplying attribute beliefs bi with their evaluations ei:Aact=i=lnbi×ei

The subjective norm is assumed to be made up of the social influence exerted by m relevant reference groups. This influence is equal to the sum of normative beliefs NBj, that is, the individual's estimate of the opinion of reference group j, weighted by MCj, the extent to which the individual is motivated to comply with these beliefs:SNact=j=lmNBj×MCj

Section snippets

Procedure

Eight hundred questionnaires were sent in two waves of 400 to the inhabitants of five cities of varying size in the central part of the Netherlands. Addressees were selected randomly from local phonebooks. Questionnaires were accompanied by an introductory letter. To increase response rate, postage-paid envelopes were enclosed and respondents were entered in a lottery, in which nine gift-vouchers (of NLG 25) were to be won. The researched behaviour was described as the consumption of TV dinners

Response

Three-hundred-and-forty (340) of the 800 questionnaires were returned, but 117 of these questionnaires could not be used. Of these questionnaires, 99 were returned unopened because the addressee had moved or deceased, and 18 were blank, or incomplete. These figures may be due to using a 3 year old phonebook for the first wave. During that part of the investigation, the national phonebook was redesigned, and an update was not available. Subtracting the 99 unopened questionnaires from the total

Discussion and suggestions for future research

The role of situational and social influences has been a neglected topic in research on food choice and preference. We studied the influence of these variables on the consumption of TV dinners by Dutch consumers. Our survey was conducted within the well-known TRA-framework (Fishbein & Ajzen, 1975). We found that the model of Fishbein and Ajzen holds reasonably well in this context. For different situations, the multiple correlation between attitude and subjective norm on the one hand and

Acknowledgments

The authors would like to thank the editor, H. Meiselman, and the anonymous reviewers for their valuable comments. Yvette van de Winkel is thanked for her assistance in collecting the data for this study.

References (30)

  • W.O. Bearden et al.

    Reference group influence on product and brand purchase decisions

  • R.W. Belk

    Situational variables and consumer behaviour

    Journal of Consumer Research

    (1975)
  • D. Brinberg et al.

    Eating at fast-food restaurantsAn analysis using two behavioral intention models

    Journal of Applied Social Psychology

    (1983)
  • Bryant, W. K. (1988). Durables and wives' employment yet again. Journal of Consumer Research, 15,...
  • O. Capps et al.

    Household demand for convenience and nonconvenience foods

    American Journal of Agricultural Economics

    (1985)
  • Cited by (77)

    • Investigating the role of personal and context-related factors in convenience foods consumption

      2018, Appetite
      Citation Excerpt :

      The role of time pressure on convenience behaviour has also been analysed by Scholderer and Grunert (2005), who have verified the importance of the perceived time and budget constraints by using a model that sums up the household production approach (Becker, 1965) and the convenience orientation approach (Candel, 2001). In the ambit of the Theory of Reasoned Action (Fishbein & Ajzen, 1975) and of the Theory of Planned Behaviour (Ajzen, 1991), several authors (Mahon, Cowan, & McCarthy, 2006; Olsen, Sijtsema, & Hall, 2010; Verlegh & Candel, 1999) have shown the influence of the social environment on convenience food consumption. Finally, the literature points out the major impact of the system of provision on what individuals eat (Steptoe, Pollard, & Wardle, 1995), identifying a relationship between an increase in the consumption of convenience foods, their variety and the number of places where they are available (de Boer et al., 2004).

    • Food habits and the increase in ready-to-eat and easy-to-prepare products

      2016, Food Hygiene and Toxicology in Ready-to-Eat Foods
    • Consumer behavior in choice of minimally processed vegetables and implications for marketing strategies

      2015, Trends in Food Science and Technology
      Citation Excerpt :

      Due to an increasing health consciousness and growing interest in the role of food for maintaining and improving human well-being and consumer health, that is now characterizing the consumer in actual society (Ballestrazzi, Mason, & Nassivera, 2011; Kher et al.,2013), vegetables are well recognized for their benefits towards healthy living (Bongoni, Steenbekkers, Verkerk, van Boekel, & Dekker, 2013; Cox et al., 1996; Wootton-Beard & Ryan, 2011), thanks to their protective function against diseases. Numerous studies have shown that consumer's needs for convenience are correlated with food choice (Hjelmar, 2011; Grunert, Brunsø, Bredahl, & Bech, 2001; Verlegh & Candel, 1999). Minimally processed vegetables has been developed to meet consumer's needs for ‘‘quick’’ products, and to benefit from vegetable and fruit's healthy image (Ahvenainen, 1996; Pula, Parks, & Ross, 2014; Vidal, Ares, & Giménez, 2013).

    View all citing articles on Scopus
    View full text