Elsevier

Physiology & Behavior

Volume 77, Issue 1, September 2002, Pages 115-123
Physiology & Behavior

Effects of the selective dopamine D1 antagonists NNC 01-0112 and SCH 39166 on latent inhibition in the rat

https://doi.org/10.1016/S0031-9384(02)00814-4Get rights and content

Abstract

Dopamine D1 receptor blockade does not appear to be a prerequisite for antipsychotic activity since many clinically effective antipsychotics have little or no affinity for this receptor subtype. Clozapine, however, which has minimal liability for extrapyramidal symptoms, possesses affinities of similar order for D1 and D2 receptors. In earlier animal models used to predict antipsychotic effect, selective D1 antagonists have shown effects similar to standard antipsychotics with preferential D2 or mixed D1/D2 antagonism. We investigated the effects of haloperidol (0.1 mg/kg) and two selective D1 antagonists, NNC 01-0112 (0.05, 0.1 and 0.2 mg/kg) and SCH 39166 (0.02, 0.2 and 2.0 mg/kg), on latent inhibition (LI) in rats. LI is a behavioural paradigm in which repeated nonreinforced preexposure to a stimulus retards subsequent associations to that stimulus. Disrupted LI has been suggested as a model for the attentional deficits in schizophrenia. Using preexposure to a flashing light stimulus, which subsequently served as a conditioned stimulus for suppression of water licking, we demonstrated a clear LI effect with haloperidol but with neither of the two D1 antagonists. Since selective D1 antagonists are not clinically effective, these results add further credibility for the relevance of LI as an animal model of psychosis.

Introduction

The clinical efficacy of classical antipsychotics has been demonstrated to be related to antagonism of the dopamine D2 receptor subtype [17], [51]. D1 receptor antagonism does not appear to be a prerequisite for antipsychotic activity, since many of these standard antipsychotics have little or no affinity for the D1 receptor. However, in spite of being efficacious in the acute and maintenance treatment of schizophrenia, the clinical value of these agents is limited by several factors: they are ineffective in treating negative symptoms, they lack clinical efficacy in ‘treatment-resistant’ patients and they produce extensive side effects including extrapyramidal symptoms (EPS). By contrast, the atypical antipsychotic clozapine has minimal EPS liability in man [32] and is efficacious in patients with negative symptoms [26], [39] and in patients who do not respond to conventional treatment [26]. Furthermore, since in vitro studies on human brain tissue have shown that clozapine possesses affinities of comparable order for D1 and D2 receptors [18], it may be the case that D1 receptor antagonism contributes to the antipsychotic effect of this compound.

SCH 23390, the prototype-selective D1 receptor antagonist, was first described by Iorio [26] as a potential antipsychotic with an atypical pharmacological profile. However, it is unsuitable for clinical development because it has a very short biological half-life in the primate [2]. A number of analogues, which possess the same pharmacological profile as SCH 23390 but which have a longer duration of action, have since been synthesised and these include SCH 39166 as well as NNC 112, NNC 687 and NNC 756 (see Ref. [1] for a review). In vitro, specific D1 antagonists bind in a saturable manner with high affinity to a single class of receptors, these receptors being selective for D1 drugs [4]. Data from preclinical studies have demonstrated that selective D1 antagonists reproduce the actions of standard antipsychotics, with preferential D2 or mixed D1 or D2 antagonism, in animal models conventionally used to predict antipsychotic activity. These models include inhibition of conditioned responses, amphetamine cue, stereotyped behaviour [8], [9], [44], [45], inhibition of intracranial self-stimulation [43] and the paw test [48].

Studies with selective D1 antagonists in nonhuman primates and rodents have produced data regarding their liability for negative symptoms and EPS. Acute administration of relatively high single doses of SCH 23390 has a comparable effect to haloperidol in that it induces dystonia in monkeys [5]. By contrast, NNC 01-0756 does not induce dystonia when administered in increasing doses, while the D2 antagonist raclopride produces dystonia at low doses [22]. In monkeys sensitised to antipsychotic treatment, NNC 22-0215 and haloperidol both produce EPS when administered acutely, but there is rapid tolerance with the D1 antagonist [6]. Furthermore, data from studies using rodents in the paw test imply that SCH 39166 has a lower potential for inducing EPS than traditional antipsychotics such as haloperidol. Such results indicate that the use of selective D1 antagonists in the clinical situation may induce EPS for a short period only as opposed to the persistent symptoms associated with chronic treatment with standard antipsychotics.

Such preclinical findings suggest that selective D1 antagonists are potential antipsychotics with less EPS than traditional antipsychotics. Unfortunately, this expectation of antipsychotic efficacy has not been fulfilled. When administered to patients, SCH 39166, the first selective D1 receptor antagonist developed for clinical trials in schizophrenia, did not induce any apparent antipsychotic effects [11], [13], [29]. No beneficial changes were found in positive symptoms with this selective D1 antagonist [11], [13], [29], although an apparent effect on negative symptoms was found by the latter study group. A preliminary study with NNC 01-0687 in chronic schizophrenic patients has reported a modest improvement in psychotic symptoms [28].

Thus, the preclinical tests produced false positives. This casts doubt upon the validity of such preclinical models used for predicting the antipsychotic activity of drugs. The aim of the present study was to investigate the effects of two selective D1 antagonists, SCH 39166 and NNC 01-0112, on the animal latent inhibition (LI) model of schizophrenia. LI has superiority over other models of schizophrenia such as amphetamine-induced hyperactivity, which have been criticised for possessing little face or construct validity [16]. These models do not bear an obvious relation to the symptoms of the disease and, in addition, do not model the psychopathological construct hypothesised to underlie the disease process. The LI model simulates an attentional deficit that is a putative central characteristic of certain forms of schizophrenia. This deficit has often been described as an inability to distinguish between relevant and irrelevant stimuli. In the LI paradigm, repeated nonreinforced preexposure to a stimulus retards subsequent associations to that stimulus. LI has been described as reflecting the process of learning to ignore irrelevant stimuli [33], [34], [35], [36] and has been demonstrated behaviourally in both animals and humans.

LI is disrupted in rats by systemic administration of amphetamine [12], [31], [52], [53], [63], [64], [65], [67]. This disruption has been suggested as a model for the attentional deficit in schizophrenia [21], [23], [42], [52], [58], [63], [64]. In human studies, acute schizophrenics show a deficit in LI, whereas chronic schizophrenics exhibit no such deficit [3], [24]. Nonschizophrenic volunteers given amphetamine also fail to show LI [25].

The validity of the LI paradigm as a model of schizophrenic attentional deficit is strengthened by the fact that antipsychotics, including both typical and atypical compounds, reverse amphetamine-induced disruption of LI and demonstrate an emergence of LI when administered alone [10], [14], [20], [21], [30], [40], [42], [47], [54], [55], [57], [59], [60], [61], [62], [66]. Significantly, in a human study employing an auditory LI task, the typical antipsychotic chlorpromazine has been demonstrated to produce an LI effect in healthy volunteers [37].

The effects of the selective D1 antagonist SCH 39166 have previously been assessed on LI, but only when this drug was administered centrally. Using the taste aversion paradigm, Ellenbroek et al. [15] demonstrated that administration of SCH 39166 into the medial prefrontal cortex had no influence on LI. The present study investigated the effects of peripheral administration of SCH 39166 (0.02, 0.2 and 2.0 mg/kg), NNC 01-0112 (0.05, 0.1 and 0.2 mg/kg) and haloperidol (0.1 mg/kg) on LI in rats. Haloperidol served as a comparator to ensure that any apparent LI effect with either of the selective D1 antagonists could be taken as reliable. The enhancing effects of haloperidol on the LI model are well established [10], [14], [21], [48], [60], [62]. Pilot experiments conducted by us have shown 0.1 mg/kg haloperidol to be the most effective dose in enhancing LI.

A three stage off-baseline conditioned emotional response procedure modelled on that of Feldon and Weiner [20] and Weiner et al. [62], [68] was employed: (1) preexposure to the to-be-conditioned stimulus (flashing houselight); (2) conditioning, during which the flashing houselight stimulus was paired with a mild footshock; and (3) test, in which LI was measured by the degree of suppression of water licking elicited by the flashing houselight stimulus. Ten preexposures were used since it has been established that using a low number of preexposures produces a small LI effect and hence a more accurate evaluation of drug influences [14], [21], [41], [59].

Section snippets

Animals

Animals were male, Sprague–Dawley rats (Laboratory Services, Medical Biology Centre, Queen's University Belfast, Belfast, UK) weighing 300–401 g (Experiment 1) and 312–402 g (Experiment 2) during the test. At 4 weeks old, they were moved to the experimental holding room and housed in groups of six/seven under a 12-h reversed cycle lighting (lights off at 0700 h). At 7/8 weeks old, they were housed two to a cage under the same reversed cycle lighting. These conditions remained the same for the

Time to complete licks 51–75

The 12 experimental groups did not differ in their times to complete licks in the period preceding stimulus presentation. A 2×6 ANOVA, with main factors of Preexposure (PE, NPE) and Drug (0.1 mg/kg haloperidol, haloperidol vehicle; 0.05, 0.1 and 0.2 mg/kg NNC 01-0112, NNC 01-0112 vehicle) yielded no significant outcomes for either the two main effects or their interaction.

Suppression ratios

The mean suppression ratios of the 12 experimental groups are illustrated in Fig. 1. The NPE and PE animals for all NNC

Discussion

In the present set of experiments, 10 stimulus preexposures resulted in no LI effect in the haloperidol, SCH 39166 and NNC 01-0112 vehicle groups. PE and NPE animals in these untreated groups did not differ in their degree of response suppression during flashing light presentation. Under the same conditions, in both experiments, an LI effect was obtained after treatment with 0.1 mg/kg haloperidol. PE animals under this drug condition exhibited less suppression of drinking in the presence of a

Acknowledgements

The authors are grateful to Schering-Plough and Novo Nordisk for the generous gifts of SCH 39166 and NNC 01-0112, respectively.

References (69)

  • S Nakajima et al.

    Reduction of the rewarding effect of brain stimulation by a blockade of dopamine D-1 receptor with SCH 23390

    Pharmacol, Biochem Behav

    (1986)
  • EB Nielsen et al.

    Dopamine receptor occupancy in vivo: behavioral correlates using NNC-112, NNC-687 and NNC-756, new selective dopamine D1 receptor antagonists

    Eur J Pharmacol

    (1992)
  • EB Nielsen et al.

    Antagonism of the amphetamine cue by both classical and atypical antipsychotic drugs

    Eur J Pharmacol

    (1985)
  • EPM Prinssen et al.

    The effects of haloperidol and raclopride in the paw test are influenced similarly by SCH 39166

    Eur J Pharmacol

    (1993)
  • KM Trimble et al.

    Enhancement of latent inhibition in the rat by the atypical antipsychotic agent remoxipride

    Pharmacol, Biochem Behav

    (1997)
  • JL Waddington

    Behavioural correlates of the action of selective D-1 dopamine receptor antagonists

    Biochem Pharmacol

    (1986)
  • I Weiner et al.

    Facilitation of the expression but not the acquisition of latent inhibition by haloperidol in rats

    Pharmacol, Biochem Behav

    (1987)
  • I Weiner et al.

    Chronic amphetamine and latent inhibition

    Behav Brain Res

    (1981)
  • I Weiner et al.

    Disruption of latent inhibition by acute administration of low doses of amphetamine

    Pharmacol, Biochem Behav

    (1988)
  • I Weiner et al.

    The latent inhibition model of schizophrenia: further validation using the atypical neuroleptic, clozapine

    Biol Psychiatry

    (1996)
  • AMJ Young et al.

    Increased dopamine release in vivo in nucleus accumbens and caudate nucleus of the rat during drinking: a microdialysis study

    Neuroscience

    (1992)
  • A Barnett

    Review on dopamine receptors

    Drugs Future

    (1986)
  • I Baruch et al.

    Differential performance of acute and chronic schizophrenics in a latent inhibition task

    J Nerv Ment Dis

    (1988)
  • DE Casey

    Dopamine D1 (SCH 23390) and D2 (haloperidol) antagonists in drug-naive monkeys

    Psychopharmacology

    (1992)
  • DE Casey

    The effects of D1 (NNC 22-0215) and D2 (haloperidol) antagonists in a chronic double-blind placebo controlled trial in cebus monkeys

    Psychopharmacology

    (1995)
  • HJ Cassaday et al.

    The effects of ritanserin, RU 24969 and 8-OH-DPAT on latent inhibition in the rat

    J Psychopharmacol

    (1993)
  • RE Chipkin et al.

    Pharmacological profile of SCH 39166: a dopamine D1 selective benzonaphthazepine with potential antipsychotic activity

    J Pharmacol Exp Ther

    (1988)
  • R de Beaurepaire et al.

    An open trial of the D1 antagonist SCH 39166 in six cases of acute psychotic states

    Psychopharmacology

    (1995)
  • JA Den Boer et al.

    Differential effects of the D1-DA receptor antagonist SCH 39166 on positive and negative symptoms of schizophrenia

    Psychopharmacology

    (1995)
  • LA Dunn et al.

    Effects of antipsychotic drugs on latent inhibition: sensitivity and specificity of an animal behavioral model of clinical drug action

    Psychopharmacology

    (1993)
  • BA Ellenbroek et al.

    Animal models with construct validity for schizophrenia

    Behav Pharmacol

    (1990)
  • L Farde et al.

    Central D2-dopamine occupancy in schizophrenic patients treated with antipsychotic drugs

    Arch Gen Psychiatry

    (1988)
  • L Farde et al.

    Positron emission tomographic analysis of central D1 and D2 receptor occupancy in patients treated with classical neuroleptics and clozapine

    Arch Gen Psychiatry

    (1992)
  • J Feldon et al.

    Long-term attentional deficit in nonhandled males: possible involvement of the dopaminergic system

    Psychopharmacology

    (1988)
  • Cited by (20)

    • Effect of dopamine and serotonin receptor antagonists on fencamfamine-induced abolition of latent inhibition

      2013, European Journal of Pharmacology
      Citation Excerpt :

      The weakness of the SCH 23390 effect on the FCF-induced abolition of latent inhibition is consistent with previous results, in which the selective dopamine D1 receptor antagonists NCC 01-01112 and SCH 39166 did not affect latent inhibition (Trimble et al., 2002) and dopamine D1 receptor agonist SKF 38393 had no influence on the formation of latent inhibition (Loskutova et al., 2010). The results are also consistent with the poor clinical response to D1 receptor antagonists (Trimble et al., 2002; Wiesel et al., 1990). The use of dopamine and serotonin receptor antagonists has been a successful strategy aimed at elucidating the participation of these neurotransmitters in the effect of FCF on latent inhibition.

    View all citing articles on Scopus
    View full text