Elsevier

Vision Research

Volume 29, Issue 10, 1989, Pages 1325-1333
Vision Research

Clinical suppression and binocular rivalry suppression: The effects of stimulus strength on the depth of suppression

https://doi.org/10.1016/0042-6989(89)90189-2Get rights and content

Abstract

In observers with abnormal binocular vision (such as strabismics or anisometropes) one eye's view is often suppressed. This clinical suppression serves to eliminate binocular diplopia and confusion. Suppression may also occur in observers with normal binocular vision, when the two eyes view disparate retinal images, a phenomenon known as binocular rivalry. When the image in an eye is suppressed, it is possible to determine the amount by which that suppressed stimulus is below the visibility threshold, or the depth of suppression. In the experiments presented here, the depth of suppression in an eye was measured as the strength of the stimulus in the contralateral eye (the stimulus inducing suppression) was varied. This was done for both clinical suppressors and normal observers undergoing binocular rivalry suppression. Independent changes were made to the contrast, the luminance, and the spatial frequency of the inducing stimulus. For both clinical suppression and binocular rivalry suppression, the depth of suppression was constant, regardless of the changes to the inducing stimulus.

Reference (20)

  • AbadiR.V.

    Induction masking—a study of some inhibitory interactions during dichoptic viewing

    Vision Research

    (1976)
  • SireteanuR. et al.

    Naso-temporal asymmetries in human amblyopia: Consequences of long-term interocular suppression

    Vision Research

    (1981)
  • BlakeR.

    A neural theory of binocular rivalry

    Psychological Review

    (1989)
  • BlakeR. et al.

    On the inhibitory nature of binocular rivalry suppression

    JEP: Human Perception and Performance

    (1979)
  • CamposE.C.

    Binocularity in comitant strabismus: binocular visual fields studies

    Documenta Ophthalmologica

    (1982)
  • Duke-ElderS. et al.
  • FahleM.

    Non-fusable stimuli and role of binocular inhibition in normal and pathological vision, especially strabismus

    Documenta Ophthalmologies

    (1983)
  • FoxR. et al.

    Binocular rivalry and reciprocal inhibition

    Perception and Psycholphysics

    (1969)
  • HolopigianK. et al.

    Clinical suppression and amblyopia

    Investigative Ophthalmology and Visual Science

    (1988)
  • Le GrandY.

    Light, colour and vision

There are more references available in the full text version of this article.

Cited by (26)

  • Individual variation in inter-ocular suppression and sensory eye dominance

    2019, Vision Research
    Citation Excerpt :

    This finding could be important in the sense that it might imply some degree of task-dependence for sensory eye dominance. This clearly suggests that although CFS and BR may share one, or more, stages of visual processing, they are not necessarily one and the same (c.f. Holopigian, 1989). However, those that showed changes in sensory eye dominance between the two tasks had considerably smaller absolute asymmetry indices (mean = 3.68 ± 1.20 for BR and 3.00 ± 0.93 for CFS) than those that did not switch (mean = 5.52 ± 1.99 for BR and 4.80 ± 1.38 for CFS).

  • Binocular retinal image differences influence eye-position signals for perceived visual direction

    2012, Vision Research
    Citation Excerpt :

    Average monocular and binocular detection thresholds for the blurred target were 0.17 ± 0.11 (SD) and 0.34 ± 0.13 cd/m2. The average depth of suppression was −0.34 ± 0.21 log units (across observers, range: −0.11 to −0.66 log units), in agreement with previous studies in strabismic subjects with suppression, and in non-human primates with experimentally induced strabismus (Holopigian, 1989; Wensween, Harwerth, & Smith, 2001). For the observers in this experiment, the fixation disparity was small, amounting to 8.95 (±5.78 SD) and 5.26 (±4.20) arcmin in the foveal-suppression and non-suppression conditions, respectively, indicating that in both conditions the asymmetric vergence response was essentially equivalent to the vergence demand.

View all citing articles on Scopus
View full text