Elsevier

Neuropsychologia

Volume 33, Issue 12, December 1995, Pages 1583-1596
Neuropsychologia

The performance of amnesic subjects on tests of delayed matching-to-sample and delayed matching-to-position

https://doi.org/10.1016/0028-3932(95)00145-XGet rights and content

Abstract

Forced-choice tests of recognition have become the favoured behavioural method for the assessment of models of amnesia in nonhuman primates, yet the profile of deficits shown by human amnesic subjects remains uncertain. The present study explored the performance of 12 amnesic subjects on two delayed matching-to-sample tasks. Experiment 1, which used retention delays of between 2 and 60 sec, confirmed that amnesia impairs such tasks, even when there is only one item to be remembered. The results also highlighted the need to match levels of performance before the effects of delay can be interpreted. In Experiment 2 care was taken to eliminate ceiling effects and to match the subjects at the shortest delay (3 sec). This was achieved by giving the control subjects harder versions of the same task. The amnesic subjects still showed a faster rate of forgetting for abstract patterns, indicating that this is a genuine feature of amnesia. In contrast, the amnesic subjects' performance on a spatial matching-to-sample task was not differentially affected by delays of up to 40 sec. There was no evidence that the amnesic subjects were disproportionately impaired on this spatial task, nor could the different aetiological groups be distinguished by their patterns of DMS performance.

References (38)

  • A.R. Mayes et al.

    Encoding ability is preserved in amnesia: Evidence from a direct test of encoding

    Neuropsychologia

    (1993)
  • A.R. Mayes et al.

    Disproportionate intentional spatial-memory impairments in amnesia

    Neuropsychologia

    (1991)
  • A.R. Mayes et al.

    Amnesic sensitivity to proactive interference: Its relationship to priming and the causes of amnesia

    Neuropsychologia

    (1987)
  • A.J. Parkin et al.

    Neuropsychological sequelae of Wernicke's encephalopathy in a 20-year-old woman: Selective impairment of a frontal memory system

    Brain Cognit.

    (1993)
  • J.L. Ringo

    Memory decays at the same rate in macaques with and without brain lesions when expressed in d' or arcisine terms

    Behav. Brain Res.

    (1991)
  • J.L. Ringo

    Spared short-term memory in monkeys following temporal lobe lesions is not yet established: A reply to Alvarez-Royo, Zola-Morgan and Squire

    Behav. Brain Res.

    (1993)
  • J.P. Aggleton et al.

    Mamillary-body lesions and visual recognition in monkeys

    Exp. Brain Res.

    (1985)
  • C.B. Cave et al.

    Equivalent impairment of spatial and nonspatial memory following damage to the human hippocampus

    Hippocampus

    (1991)
  • H. Dusoir et al.

    The role of diencephalic pathology in human memory disorder

    Brain

    (1990)
  • Cited by (67)

    • Automatic and effortful control of interference in working memory can be distinguished by unique behavioral and functional brain representations

      2022, NeuroImage
      Citation Excerpt :

      There is much evidence showing that amnesic patients can retain information over brief delays despite severe deficits in long-term memory (Baddeley et al., 2010; Jeneson et al., 2010, 2011, 2012; Shrager et al., 2008). While it has been demonstrated that these deficits become larger with increasing time between encoding and memory testing (Aggleton et al., 1992; Buffalo et al., 1998; Holdstock et al., 1995,2000; Owen et al., 1995), damage to the MTL can produce memory deficits with retention intervals as short as 2–10 s (Hannula et al., 2006). Our results are in line with the view that memory is best thought of as a continuum where MTL is increasingly critical as retention intervals increases.

    • Mapping working memory retrieval in space and in time: A combined electroencephalography and electrocorticography approach

      2018, NeuroImage
      Citation Excerpt :

      The observation of frontal-MTL theta coupling generalizes the role of MTL theta from retrieval in episodic memory to retrieval of previous items in working memory. This is consistent with recent studies that MTL is not uniquely involved in long-term memory, but also critical to short-term memory even when the retention period is as short as 2–10s (Holdstock et al., 1995; Owen et al., 1995; Holdstock et al., 2000; Aggleton et al., 1992; Hannula et al., 2006, Van Vugt et al., 2010). There are several limitations to our research.

    • Hierarchical process memory: Memory as an integral component of information processing

      2015, Trends in Cognitive Sciences
      Citation Excerpt :

      Hippocampal damage strikingly impacts the ability to retain episodic memories [68], but exactly how long new information can be maintained in cortical areas without hippocampal involvement is not clear. For example, hippocampal amnesics can retain stimulus information for long enough to engage in a conversation [68], summarize a short passage of prose [69,70], and play a complex communicative game [71]. Such observations suggest that a meaningful continuous context (e.g., a conversation or listening to a story) may enable information to persist in cortical areas for a few minutes without relying on the hippocampus (though hippocampal circuits do appear to contribute to ongoing processing when they are intact [72–74]).

    • Working memory and amnesia: The role of stimulus novelty

      2012, Neuropsychologia
      Citation Excerpt :

      The proposal that the MTL is especially important for spatial binding is consistent with numerous findings of impaired performance by amnesics on a variety of visuospatial WM tasks (Buffalo et al., 1998; Crane & Milner, 2005; Ezzyat & Olson, 2008; Hannula et al., 2006; Holdstock et al., 1995; Olson et al., 2006; Owen et al., 1995; Shrager, Levy, Hopkins, & Squire, 2008; Warrington & Taylor, 1973). Although it is true that many of the WM tasks showing impairments in amnesia required spatial binding, we would also note that many of them involved novel stimuli, such as abstract patterns resembling images from a kaleidoscope (Buffalo et al., 1998), or abstract shapes resembling snowflakes (Holdstock et al., 1995) or letters of a foreign language (Owen et al., 1995). Therefore, it is likely that the hippocampus is important for WM tasks that require binding features among novel stimuli, not solely tasks that require spatial binding (see Experiment 2).

    View all citing articles on Scopus
    View full text