Religion and child development: Evidence from the Early Childhood Longitudinal Study

https://doi.org/10.1016/j.ssresearch.2007.02.001Get rights and content

Abstract

Although interest in religion among young people has increased markedly, research examining the impact of religion on child development has been sorely lacking. This study is the first of its kind to use nationally representative data to explore the influence of religion on several different dimensions of psychological development and social adjustment in early childhood. Data from the Spring 2000 wave of the Early Childhood Longitudinal Study (ECLS-K) are analyzed to explore how child development is shaped by individual parents’ religiosity, the religious homogamy of couples, and the family’s religious environment. Significant religious effects are observed across a range of child development domains and are manifested for both parents’ ratings and teachers’ ratings of youngsters. Parental, couple, and familial religion are linked with youngsters’ pro-social behavior. However, religion can undermine child development when it is a source of conflict among families. The investigation concludes with a specification of implications and directions for future research.

Introduction

Is parental religiosity a blessing or a bane for young children? Does religion facilitate or undermine the psychological development and social adjustment of youngsters? While no scholarship has offered a direct answer to such questions, countervailing evidence surfaces from related bodies of literature. To date, a great deal of research has surfaced concerning religious variations in parental values, child-rearing practices, paternal involvement, and parental attachment (e.g., Alwin, 1986, Bartkowski, 1995, Bartkowski and Ellison, 1995, Bartkowski and Wilcox, 2000, Bartkowski and Xu, 2000, Dollahite, 1998, Dollahite, 2003, Ellison et al., 1996a, Ellison et al., 1996b, Ellison and Sherkat, 1993a, Ellison and Sherkat, 1993b, King, 2003, Mahoney et al., 2003, Pearce and Axin, 1998, Wilcox, 1998, Wilcox, 2002; see Ellison, 1996, Bartkowski et al., 2000 for reviews). On the positive side of the ledger, religion has been shown to enhance the parent–child bond for both mothers (Pearce and Axin, 1998) and fathers (Bartkowski and Xu, 2000, King, 2003, Wilcox, 2002, Wilcox, 2004). Moreover, scholarship on religion and youth has demonstrated that faith is generally important to American teens, and that religion reduces adolescents’ involvement in risk activities while fostering pro-social behaviors (e.g., Miller et al., 1997, Pearce et al., 2003, Smith, 2003a, Smith, 2003b, Smith, 2003c, Smith and Denton, 2005, Smith et al., 2002, Smith and Faris, 2002a, Smith and Faris, 2002b, Smith et al., 2003, Smith and Kim, 2003, Regnerus et al., 2003). Thus, religion would seem to function as a positive influence in the lives of youth.

At the same time, observers have expressed concern at the disciplinary tactics used in some religious families, most notably, conservative Protestants households (see Bartkowski, 1995, Ellison, 1996 for reviews). Conservative Protestants have been shown to punish their children physically much more frequently than other parents, demonstrate higher levels of attitudinal support for the use of a “good, hard spanking” as a legitimate means of discipline, and tend to value obedience rather than autonomy in youngsters (Ellison and Sherkat, 1993a, Ellison and Sherkat, 1993b, Ellison et al., 1996a, Ellison et al., 1996b, Gershoff et al., 1999, Grasmick et al., 1991). Subsequent research has revealed, rather paradoxically, that more spanking in conservative Protestant homes is coupled with less yelling, more positive emotion work (i.e., hugging and praising of children), and higher levels of paternal involvement (Bartkowski and Xu, 2000, Bartkowski and Wilcox, 2000, Wilcox, 1998, Wilcox, 2002, Wilcox, 2004). Nevertheless, given the sizable literature that links spanking to negative developmental outcomes for children (Straus, 1994), concerns have been raised about the developmental deficits that conservative Protestant youngsters would seem to face during childhood and adolescence as a result of the ostensibly strict discipline to which they are subject early in life (see Bartkowski, 1995 for review). Published empirical evidence on the effects of conservative Protestant child discipline, however, is lacking.

This conflicting evidence notwithstanding, religion and spirituality appear to be an important part of many children’s lives and are vital to family relationships. People’s images of God are formed early in the life course (e.g., Richert and Barrett, 2005, Tirri et al., 2005, Nucci and Turiel, 1993; see Boyatzis, 2003) and relationships to parents are particularly influential in shaping youngsters’ spiritual formation (e.g., Hertel and Donahue, 1995, Ratcliff, 1992; see Mahoney et al., 2003). In various ways, then, religion seems to “sanctify” family relationships (Mahoney, 2005, Mahoney et al., 2001, Mahoney et al., 2003; see also Marks, 2004, Pargament and Mahoney, 2005). Through such dictums as “train up a child in the way he or she should go,” religious norms encourage parents to invest a great deal of quantity and quality time in their children (Bartkowski, 2001, Bartkowski and Wilcox, 2000, Bartkowski and Xu, 2000, Mahoney et al., 2003, Pearce and Axin, 1998, Wilcox, 2004). Furthermore, religious communities provide a meaning system (or “nomos”) that imbues family relationships with spiritual, enduring significance which commonly includes divine accountability for the discharge of parental obligations (Mahoney et al., 2003, Wilcox, 1998, Wilcox, 2002, Wilcox, 2004). And, finally, social networks in religious congregations (e.g., parents’ Sunday school classes and scripture study groups) provide sanctions, both positive and negative, designed to encourage parents to attend to their obligations as caregivers (Bartkowski, 2001, Wilcox, 2004; see Bartkowski and Xu, 2007). Thus, the norms, nomos, and networks found in religious communities combine to create a distinctive context for familial relationships and functioning.

This constellation of factors is well articulated in the work of Mahoney and colleagues (2003, p. 221), who argue: “Religion is distinctive because it incorporates peoples’ perceptions of the ‘sacred’ into the search for significant goals and values  The sacred refers to the holy, those things that are ‘set apart’ from the ordinary and deserve veneration and respect  Indeed, part of the power of religion lies in its ability to infuse spiritual character and significance into a broad range of worldly concerns,” including those in the domestic arena. In this sense, religious involvement can be understood as a cultural resource that a family can use to enhance cohesion among its members, resolve conflicts that may arise, and identify and pursue collectively desired goals (Marks, 2004, Mahoney, 2005). Mahoney and colleagues contend that religious people “view family relationships as sacred  Judeo-Christian religions portray the burdens and pleasures of parenting as opportunities to model and deepen one’s understanding of God’s love, patience, and commitment, and frame the parental role as a sacred calling that requires personal sacrifices” (Mahoney et al., 2003, pp. 222–223). Thus, religious parenting can take on a covenantal character, and caregivers can draw on the developmental resources that faith communities provide when confronting the challenges that parenting presents (Wagener et al., 2003). And yet, Mahoney et al. (2003) recognize that religious sanctification may not always be positive. It is possible that religion could serve as a source of conflict (rather than cohesion) in some families, such as those where spouses do not share the same faith commitments (Mahoney, 2005).

Regardless of the precise ways in which religion sanctifies family relationships, American households have ample access to the resources that faith communities provide. Ninety-five percent of married couples and parents in the United States claim a religious affiliation, while 60% of women and 53% of men, respectively, attend religious services once per month or more frequently (Glenn, 1982; see Mahoney et al., 2001). Over 4 in 10 Americans believe the Bible has answers to all important human problems (Heaton and Pratt, 1990; see Mahoney et al., 2001).

Despite the burgeoning recognition of religion’s sanctification of family relationships and the pervasive presence of religion in American homes, scholarship directly examining the role of religion in child development has been sorely lacking.2 Thus, while ample attention has been paid to the generally positive influence of religion on adolescent outcomes (Bridges and Moore, 2002), scholars have yet to address the extent to which religion bolsters or undermines the developmental trajectories of young children. In fact, the sole published study of religion and child development was conducted with data drawn from a convenience sample of parents and children, the latter of whom were preschoolers enrolled in a Head Start program (Strayhorn et al., 1990). Significantly, the authors of this study called attention to the non-existent prior research on religion and child development in the introduction to their investigation (Strayhorn et al., 1990, p. 35), a fact that remains many years after the study’s publication (Bridges and Moore, 2002).

Respondents in the Strayhorn et al. (1990) study were predominantly black, low-income mothers of Head Start enrollees. Parental religiosity was measured through a twelve-item scale adapted from previously published research (e.g., frequency of prayer, scripture study, and worship service attendance).3 Children’s behavior was gauged by a number of measures, including researchers’ ratings of videotaped parent–child interactions and researchers’ assessments of children’s classroom behaviors using a preschool behavior assessment survey. While the sampling design in this study was less than ideal from a sociological perspective (i.e., a nonrandom, unrepresentative sample), multiple measures of parental religiosity and multiple contexts for data collection (videotaped parent–child interactions, children’s classroom behavior) were strong points of the investigation. Strayhorn et al. (1990) found parental religiosity to be associated with a number of positive factors for adults (e.g., parental mental health, social support, and positive parenting practices), but unassociated with children’s behavior.

Section snippets

Study foci and anticipated contributions

To date, the extent to which religion helps or hurts the development of young children has eluded social scientists. Under what circumstances does faith promote or, alternatively, undermine the psychological adjustment and social competence of young children? Data limitations, likely fueled by a dearth of interest in religion exhibited by child development scholars (Bridges and Moore, 2002), have left this important question unanswered, with the exception of the single study noted above (

Data description

This study utilizes data from the Early Childhood Longitudinal Study-Kindergarten Class (ECLS-K). The ECLS-K is a multi-source, multi-method study that focuses on children’s early childhood and school experiences beginning with kindergarten. Sponsored by the U.S. Department of Education and National Center for Education Statistics, the initial wave of ECLS-K data was collected from a nationally representative sample of 21,260 kindergarteners and first-graders, as well as their parents and

Results

Descriptive statistics for all variables prior to listwise deletion are presented in Table 1. For each variable, the sample size (n) and percent or mean and standard deviation are presented.

Table 2 features the results of a first series of regression equations. Unstandardized coefficients and significance levels are reported. In each table, the models are presented such that Model 1 features the net effects (i.e., estimated effects net of control variables) of fathers’ religious attendance on

Discussion

Is religion good for young children? How does parental religiosity, the degree to which couples share faith commitments, and the religious environment in the home influence the behavioral, emotional, and cognitive development of young children? This study set out to correct a major gap in current scholarship on this topic by analyzing nationally representative data from the Early Childhood Longitudinal Study (ECLS-K). Previous scholarship has highlighted the protective effects of religion for

References (63)

  • L. Miller et al.

    Religiosity and depression: ten-year follow-up of depressed mothers and offspring

    Journal of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry

    (1997)
  • D.F. Alwin

    Religion and parental childrearing orientations: evidence of a Catholic–Protestant convergence

    American Journal of Sociology

    (1986)
  • J.P. Bartkowski

    Spare the rod…, or spare the child? Divergent perspectives on conservative Protestant child discipline

    Review of Religious Research

    (1995)
  • J.P. Bartkowski

    Remaking the Godly Marriage: Gender Negotiation in Evangelical Families

    (2001)
  • J.P. Bartkowski et al.

    Divergent models of childrearing: conservative Protestants vs the mainstream experts

    Sociology of Religion

    (1995)
  • J.P. Bartkowski et al.

    Conservative Protestant child discipline: the case of parental yelling

    Social Forces

    (2000)
  • J.P. Bartkowski et al.

    Distant patriarchs or expressive dads? The discourse and practice of fathering in conservative Protestant families

    The Sociological Quarterly

    (2000)
  • Bartkowski, J.P., Xu, X., 2007. Religion and teen drug use reconsidered: a social capital perspective. American Journal...
  • J.P. Bartkowski et al.

    Charting the paradoxes of evangelical family life: gender and parenting in conservative Protestant households

    Family Ministry

    (2000)
  • C. Boyatzis

    Religious and spiritual development: an introduction

    Review of Religious Research

    (2003)
  • C. Boyatzis et al.

    Parent–child communication about religion: survey and diary data on unilateral transmission and bi-directional reciprocity styles

    Review of Religious Research

    (2003)
  • L.J. Bridges et al.

    Religion and Spirituality in Childhood and Adolescence

    (2002)
  • J. Chinitz et al.

    Religious homogamy, marital conflict, and stability in same-faith and interfaith Jewish marriages

    Journal for the Scientific Study of Religion

    (2001)
  • K.T. Curtis et al.

    Religious heterogamy and marital conflict: findings from the National Survey of Families and Households

    Journal of Family Issues

    (2002)
  • D.C. Dollahite

    Fathering, faith, and spirituality

    Journal of Men’s Studies

    (1998)
  • D.C. Dollahite

    Fathering for eternity: generative spirituality in Latter-day Saint fathers of children with special needs

    Review of Religious Research

    (2003)
  • C.G. Ellison

    Conservative Protestantism and the corporal punishment of children: clarifying the issues

    Journal for the Scientific Study of Religion

    (1996)
  • C.G. Ellison et al.

    Evangelical Protestantism and the household division of labor among married couples

    Journal of Family Issues

    (2002)
  • C.G. Ellison et al.

    Obedience and autonomy: religion and parental values reconsidered

    Journal for the Scientific Study of Religion

    (1993)
  • C.G. Ellison et al.

    Conservative Protestantism and support for corporal punishment

    American Sociological Review

    (1993)
  • C.G. Ellison et al.

    Conservative Protestantism and the parental use of corporal punishment

    Social Forces

    (1996)
  • C.G. Ellison et al.

    Do conservative Protestant parents spank more often? Further evidence from the National Survey of Families and Households

    Social Science Quarterly

    (1996)
  • C.G. Ellison et al.

    Are there religious variations in spousal violence?

    Journal of Family Issues

    (1999)
  • E.T. Gershoff et al.

    Parenting influences from the pulpit: religious affiliation as a determinant of parental corporal punishment

    Journal of Family Psychology

    (1999)
  • J. Glass et al.

    Childhood religious conservatism and adult attainment among black and white women

    Social Forces

    (2005)
  • N.D. Glenn

    Interreligious marriage in the United States: patterns and recent trends

    Journal of Marriage and Family

    (1982)
  • H. Grasmick et al.

    Protestant fundamentalism and attitudes toward corporal punishment of children

    Violence and Victims

    (1991)
  • T.B. Heaton

    Religious homogamy and marital satisfaction reconsidered

    Journal of Marriage and Family

    (1984)
  • T.B. Heaton et al.

    The effects of religious homogamy on marital satisfaction and stability

    Journal of Family Issues

    (1990)
  • B.R. Hertel et al.

    Parental influences on God images among children: testing Durkheim’s metaphoric parallelism

    Journal for the Scientific Study of Religion

    (1995)
  • V. King

    The influence of religion on fathers’ relationships with their children

    Journal of Marriage and Family

    (2003)
  • Cited by (151)

    View all citing articles on Scopus

    This research was generously supported by a grant from the Early Childhood Institute at Mississippi State University. A version of this paper was presented at the 2004 annual meetings of the Southern Sociological Society in Atlanta, Georgia. The authors thank Michael McCullough as well as the editor and reviewers of Social Science Research for thoughtful comments on a previous version of this paper. The authors claim full responsibility for all analyses and interpretations.

    1

    Present address: Department of Sociology, University of Memphis, Clement Hall, Room 231 B, Memphis, TN 38152.

    View full text