Elsevier

Animal Behaviour

Volume 74, Issue 4, October 2007, Pages 689-698
Animal Behaviour

Articles
Behavioural syndromes in brook charr, Salvelinus fontinalis: prey-search in the field corresponds with space use in novel laboratory situations

https://doi.org/10.1016/j.anbehav.2007.01.009Get rights and content

Recently emerged brook charr foraging in still-water pools along the sides of streams tend to be either sedentary, feeding from the lower portion of the water column (a sit-and-wait tactic) near the stream bank, or very active, feeding from the upper portion of the water column (an active search tactic) away from the bank. We tested whether the search tactics used by charr in the field represent behavioural syndromes related to activity and space use. After quantifying the behaviour of fish in the field, focal individuals were captured and their behaviour quantified in novel environment experiments in the laboratory. In an aquarium, individuals that used an active search tactic in the field spent a higher proportion of time moving, spent less time near the aquarium bottom, and took less time to find their way out of an erect glass jar, on average, than did individuals that used a sit-and-wait tactic in the field. When presented with near-bank and open-water conditions over 6 days in the laboratory, individuals that used an active search tactic in the field remained active and altered their activity less, on average, than individuals that used a sit-and-wait tactic in the field. Immediate responses to a pebble dropped in the aquarium (simulated risk from above) were not correlated with field behaviour. The search tactics used by brook charr in the field reflect part of a behavioural syndrome related to general activity and space use, but not to startle responses. These initial, individual differences in behaviour provide important raw material for the initial stages of resource polymorphism.

Section snippets

Field Observations

Between 5 April and 16 May 2004, we observed and captured 89 young-of-the-year (YOY) brook charr (fork length: 2.0–3.0 cm; weight: 0.050–0.300 g) from the west branch of the Credit River (mean water temperature: ∼10°C) in Erin Township, Ontario. Observations were made from seven still-water pools (surface area: ∼1.0–1.5 m2) along a 1-km stretch of river and took place between 0900 and 1200 hours (Eastern Standard Time).

For a given observation period, an observer arrived at a pool and, while

Experiment 1: Latency to Exit a Jar

Fish that were more active in the field took less time to escape from the glass jar than those that were less active in the field (G1 = 5.77, P < 0.02). The time-to-event (jar exit) regression predicted that the most active individuals from the field would have exit times that were 30% lower, on average, than those of the least active fish from the field (Fig. 2). When individual fish moved, the nature of the movement was consistent with that shown by active, foraging fish in the field. No fish

Discussion

Our study supports three main conclusions. First, the sit-and-wait and active search tactics used by brook charr in the field reflect part of a behavioural syndrome encompassing general activity and space use (experiments 1, 2 and 4), but not startle responses (experiment 3). Second, activity of charr in the laboratory was shaped both by flexible responses to environmental conditions and by underlying predispositions to be active (experiments 2 and 4) and the predispositions were repeatable for

Acknowledgments

We thank four anonymous referees, Drs D. Noakes, N. Bernier, J. Fryxell and E. Boulding for comments on earlier drafts of this manuscript, S. Burosch for assistance in the field and laboratory, R. Frank and M. Cornish for technical support in Hagen Aqualab, and the Ontario Ministry of Natural Resources (OMNR) for a permit to collect fish. This research was supported by an NSERC Operating Grant to R. L. McLaughlin and was approved by University of Guelph Animal Care Committee (Animal Utilization

References (67)

  • V.L. Birt-Friesen et al.

    The activity-specific metabolic rates of free-living northern gannets and other seabirds

    Ecology

    (1989)
  • A. Boissy

    Fear and fearfulness in animals

    Quarterly Review of Biology

    (1995)
  • D.I. Bolnick

    Can intraspecific variation competition drive disruptive selection? An experimental test in natural populations of sticklebacks

    Evolution

    (2004)
  • D.I. Bolnick et al.

    The ecology of individuals: incidence and implications of individual specialization

    American Naturalist

    (2003)
  • J.E. Bryan

    Feeding history, parental stock, and food selection in rainbow trout

    Behaviour

    (1973)
  • J.E. Bryan et al.

    Food specialization by individual trout

    Journal of the Fisheries Research Board of Canada

    (1972)
  • Coombs, M. 2004. Juvenile brook charr (Salvelinus fontinalis Mitchill) dispersal is compatible with simple diffusion...
  • C.J. Cutts et al.

    Aggression and growth depression in juvenile Atlantic salmon: the consequences of individual variation in standard metabolic rate

    Journal of Fish Biology

    (1998)
  • S.R.X. Dall et al.

    The behavioural ecology of personality: consistent individual differences from an adaptive perspective

    Ecology Letters

    (2004)
  • D. De Kerckhove et al.

    Ecological mechanisms favouring behavioural diversification in the absence of morphological diversification: a theoretical examination using brook charr (Salvelinus fontinalis)

    Journal of Animal Ecology

    (2006)
  • N.J. Dingemanse et al.

    Natural selection and animal personality

    Behaviour

    (2005)
  • N.J. Dingemanse et al.

    Natal dispersal and personalities in great tits (Parus major)

    Proceedings of the Royal Society of London, Series B

    (2003)
  • L.A. Dugatkin et al.

    Boldness, behavioral inhibition and learning

    Ethology, Ecology and Evolution

    (2003)
  • J. Dynes et al.

    Genetic and morphological variation between two forms of lacustrine brook charr

    Journal of Fish Biology

    (1999)
  • D.F. Fraser et al.

    Explaining leptokurtic movement distributions: intrapopulation variation in boldness and exploration

    American Naturalist

    (2001)
  • J.G.J. Godin et al.

    Female mating preference for bold males in the guppy, Poecilia reticulata

    Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, U.S.A.

    (1996)
  • S.D. Gosling

    From mice to men: what can we learn about personality from animal research

    Psychological Bulletin

    (2001)
  • J.W.A. Grant et al.

    Movers and stayers: foraging tactics of young-of-the-year brook charr, Salvelinus fontinalis

    Journal of Animal Ecology

    (1987)
  • J.W.A. Grant et al.

    Escape behaviour and use of cover by young-of-the-year brook trout, Salvelinus fontinalis

    Canadian Journal of Fisheries and Aquatic Sciences

    (1987)
  • M. Hollander et al.

    Nonparametric Statistical Methods

    (1999)
  • D.W. Hosmer et al.

    Applied Survival Analysis: Regression Modeling of Time to Event Data

    (1999)
  • D.W. Hosmer et al.

    Applied Logistic Regression

    (2000)
  • F. Huntingford et al.

    Behavioural syndromes in farmed fish: implications for production and welfare

    Behaviour

    (2005)
  • Cited by (97)

    • Freshwater fish personalities in the Anthropocene

      2024, Ecohydrology and Hydrobiology
    • The individuality affects the hormone secretion of the caudal neurosecretory system (CNSS) and correlation with respiratory metabolism, spontaneous activity and appetite in olive flounder (Paralichthys olivaceus)

      2022, Aquaculture
      Citation Excerpt :

      Moreover, BI have a higher feeding tendency, and are found to be more likely to find food (Ioannou et al., 2008; Jolles et al., 2016). Our results are consistent with other evidence that BI are prone to adapt to and explore new environments quickly, while SI are more likely to retreat or become cautious (Wilson and McLaughlin, 2007). In the respiratory metabolism experiment, our results suggest that the standard metabolic rate (SMR), maximum metabolic rate (MMR), and absolute aerobic scope (AAS) of BI were clearly higher than those of SI, which is in line with the finding that bold juvenile bluegill sunfish showed more robust aerobic capacity than SI (Binder et al., 2016).

    • Differential gene expression associated with behavioral variation in ecotypes of Lake Superior brook trout (Salvelinus fontinalis)

      2021, Comparative Biochemistry and Physiology - Part D: Genomics and Proteomics
      Citation Excerpt :

      Our sampling window was designed to capture the time points when brook trout begin to exhibit differences in feeding behavior (Wilson and McLaughlin, 2007). These two feeding strategies – sit-and-wait and actively foraging – are connected to underlying variation in several behaviors including activity and risk-taking (Wilson and McLaughlin, 2007; Farwell and McLaughlin, 2009) and, potentially, be connected to the propensity to migrate. Although it is clear that patterns of gene expression did not cluster with respect to behavior (Fig. 2), the data generated herein allowed us to investigate the link between differential expression and behavioral development.

    • “How not to judge a deer by its cover”: A personality assessment study on captive adult red deer males (Cervus elaphus)

      2021, Behavioural Processes
      Citation Excerpt :

      In animal behavior science, these between-individual differences were traditionally considered a “noisy” variation around an adaptive population mean; however, during the last decades, statistical models have developed such that this variation can be quantified (Araya-Ajoy and Dingemanse, 2014; Dingemanse and Dochtermann, 2013; Dingemanse et al., 2010; Martin et al., 2019). Furthermore, personality has been linked to consistent inter-individual variation in ecologically relevant situations (Réale and Montiglio, 2020), for instance foraging (Wilson and McLaughlin, 2007), dispersal (Dingemanse and de Goede, 2004), acquisition of dominance ranks (Fox et al., 2009), group joining preferences (Harcourt et al., 2009) or cognition (Boogert et al., 2018). Researchers have identified several important axes of animal personality in which individuals can be placed in a wide range of organisms, from mammals to birds, reptiles, fish and invertebrates (Gosling, 2001; Carere and Maestripieri, 2013).

    View all citing articles on Scopus
    1

    R. L. McLaughlin is at the Department of Integrative Biology, University of Guelph, Guelph ON, N1G 2W1, Canada.

    View full text