Introduction

Breast cancer is a leading cause of death among Hispanic women in the USA [1]. Risk-based screening tailored to an individual’s genetic, medical, and socio-behavioral characteristics has the potential to reduce disparities among Hispanic women [2]. Leading organizations, including the US Preventive Services Task Force, the American College of Radiology, and legislative mandates recommend the use of risk-based vs age-based approaches to direct women at high risk for breast cancer to appropriate supplemental screening and risk-reduction strategies [3,4,5,6]. Despite availability of evidence-based risk reduction screening strategies, Hispanic women are more likely to be diagnosed at a younger age, with advanced stage disease, and have a lower 5-year survival rate compared to non-Hispanic, White women [7,8,9,10]. These disparities have been attributed to complex factors including lack of knowledge of cancer and screening recommendations, lack of access to and use of mammography screening services, fear, and lack of insurance and/or transportation [11,12,13,14,15,16].

Central to the successful implementation of risk-based screening approaches are women’s understanding of their personal risk for breast cancer. Risk perceptions are complex, contextual evaluations of knowledge that often motivate women to engage in preventive and screening behaviors [17, 18]. Perception of risk for a particular outcome such as breast cancer is often informed by a combination of factors including one’s prior screening experience [19]. However, many women misperceive their risk for breast cancer [20,21,22], and prior studies show that Hispanic women often perceive their breast cancer risk as low [23,24,25]. Inaccurate perceptions of personal risk could lead to inappropriate screening schedules and missed opportunities to utilize risk reduction options such as genetic counseling and chemoprevention [4,5,6, 26].

While the relationship between perceived risk and screening behavior is well documented, few studies have explored factors associated with breast cancer risk perceptions among primarily Spanish-speaking, Hispanic women who are more susceptible to disparities in breast cancer screening and mortality. Thus, the purpose of this study is to understand the associations between patient factors including sociodemographic, clinical, and prior experience, on Hispanic women’s perceived risk for breast cancer.

Methods

This is a secondary analysis of self-reported baseline survey data from an NIH-funded randomized trial comparing the impact of three breast density educational approaches on behavioral and psychological outcomes [27]. Eligible women in the trial were English or Spanish-speaking Hispanic women, between 40 and 74 years of age, presenting for a screening mammogram at a large Federally Qualified Health Center (FQHC) in Phoenix, AZ. FQHCs are safety net clinics that provide comprehensive healthcare services including primary and preventive care regardless of a patient’s ability to pay [28]. Between October 2016 and October 2019, 1332 Hispanic women were registered and completed the baseline survey. For this analysis, we excluded 7 women who did not respond to any of the self-perceived risk questions or the mammography screening behavior questions (N = 1325). This study was approved by the Mayo Clinic Institutional Review Board and all participants provided written informed consent and received financial compensation for their time.

Data Collection

The baseline survey was administered in-person during a screening appointment and included items on demographic characteristics, family history of breast cancer, mammography screening behavior, health literacy, reproductive history (e.g., parity, age at menarche, menopausal status), and health beliefs including perceived risk of breast cancer. Clinical characteristics from the electronic health record, such as body mass index (BMI), primary family history of breast cancer (first-degree relative with breast cancer), and history of breast biopsy, were also collected at baseline.

Perceived Risk of Breast Cancer

The baseline survey included three common measures of perceived risk of breast cancer adapted from prior studies with Hispanics and nationally representative surveys including the Health Information National Trends Survey [29,30,31]. A numerical risk estimation was obtained by asking participants to estimate their risk of breast cancer in their lifetime using a 0–100% open-ended response (percent lifetime risk) [29, 31]. Due to sparse distribution in participant responses (59% of respondents assigned themselves a risk of 0%, 75% self-reported a risk of 0–10%), we dichotomized responses as 0–10% and > 10% to align with current estimates of a Hispanic woman’s lifetime risk of developing breast cancer (9.8%) [32]. Ordinal lifetime risk was assessed by asking participants to rate their lifetime risk on a 5-point ordinal scale [29, 31]. Responses were dichotomized as “very low, moderately low, neither high nor low” and “moderately high or very high.” Participants were also asked to assess their risk compared to women their age with the response options of “much lower,” “about the same,” and “much higher” (comparative risk) [30, 31].

Mammography Screening History

We defined mammography screening behavior as the number of prior mammograms completed prior to baseline. Participants were asked if they ever had a mammogram before their screening appointment with responses dichotomized as “yes” and “no.” Women responding “yes” were then asked how many total mammograms they think they had in their lifetime. Response options were categorized as “0” (including those with no prior mammogram), “1,” “2–4,” and “5 + .”

Analysis

Given that guideline recommendations for initiation and frequency of mammography screening are largely driven by age, we divided the study population into two cohorts, namely, those less than 50 years of age (< 50) and those aged 50 or older (50 +) at study entry. For each cohort, chi-square and Kruskal–Wallis tests (for categorical and ordinal variables, respectively) were used to examine whether any of three measures of perceived risk of breast cancer (percent lifetime, percent ordinal, comparative) differed by patient social demographic or breast cancer risk factors. Multivariate analyses were performed with logistic regression, using the score method for model selection, to examine the association between mammography history and percent perceived lifetime risk (outcome > 10 vs ≤ 10%) for the overall group and each cohort. The multivariate analysis was limited to perceived lifetime risk due to a small number of women perceiving their ordinal and comparative risk as high. Odds ratios (OR), along with 95% confidence intervals (CI), are reported, and p-values less than 0.05 were considered statistically significant. All analyses were performed using SAS version 9.4 (SAS Institute Inc., Cary, NC).

Results

Table 1 provides demographic and screening history of the women in our study population. Approximately two-thirds of the women were under the age of 50 (64%) and 69% had less than a high school education. Over 90% of the women completed the survey in Spanish, and 92% never had a breast biopsy or a primary family history of breast cancer. In our sample, perceived risk for breast cancer was low, with 75% considering their percent lifetime risk between 0 and 10%, 96% stating their ordinal risk as “not high,” and 50% describing their comparative risk as “much lower” than peers of similar age. The distributions of lifetime risk, comparative risk, and ordinal lifetime overall and by cohort are presented in Fig. 1. In general, ratings of perceived risk were similarly across all measures. Yet, there are still women who perceived their lifetime risk to be ≤ 10% but consider their ordinal and comparative risk as high.

Table 1 Summary of participant characteristics (N = 1325)
Fig. 1
figure 1

Alignment between three measures of perceived risk overall and by age

Among women under the age of 50 (Table 2), those who considered themselves to have a higher level of risk were significantly more likely to report a primary family history of breast cancer than those reporting lower levels of risk (all three measures, p < 0.01). For percent lifetime risk and comparative risk endpoints, but not for ordinal lifetime risk, women who considered themselves to have a higher level of risk were significantly more likely to report English as their preferred language or had a history of breast biopsy than those reporting lower levels of risk (all p < 0.05). For the percent lifetime risk endpoint only, women stating their breast cancer risk as > 10% were more likely to have finished high school as compared to those stating risk of 0–10% (p < 0.01). Among women aged 50 or older (Table 3), those who reported higher levels of perceived risk across all three measures were also significantly more likely to speak English as their primary language (p < 0.05). For the percent lifetime risk and ordinal lifetime risk endpoints, women who considered themselves to have a higher level of risk were significantly more likely to report a primary family history of breast cancer than those reporting lower levels of risk (p < 0.01).

Table 2 Association of sociodemographic, clinical, and prior experience on perceived risk of breast cancer age < 50
Table 3 Association of sociodemographic, clinical, and prior experience on perceived risk of breast cancer age ≥ 50

Next, we examined associations with the likelihood of a woman perceiving her risk of developing breast cancer as > 10% (Table 4). No statistically significant association was found between number of prior mammograms and the likelihood a woman perceives her risk of developing breast cancer as > 10% after adjusting for age, education, primary language, and a primary family history of breast cancer. Examining this question in the cohort of women < 50 years of age as well as the cohort of women ≥ 50 years of age revealed no statistically significant association between number of prior mammograms and the likelihood a woman perceives her risk of developing breast cancer as > 10% after adjusting for education level attained and primary family history of breast cancer.

Table 4 Results of logistic regression modeling the odds of perceived risk (> 10 vs 0–10%) based on mammography screening history behavior by age

Discussion

In this study, we aimed to identify factors associated with perceived risk for breast cancer in a largely understudied population and to assess the relationship between mammography screening history and perceived risk. Overall, Hispanic women’s perceptions of their breast cancer risk was low, and we observed no significant associations between mammography screening history and percent perceived lifetime risk of breast cancer. Our findings are consistent with the limited number of studies inclusive of Hispanic women [23, 25, 30, 33], and may reflect issues in appropriate and acceptable cancer risk and prevention messaging, greater presence of competing risks, and salience of risk perceptions [30, 34].

Lived experiences are believed to play a key role in constructing risk perceptions. For instance, having consecutive normal mammogram results may be interpreted by the patient as a clean bill of health and evidence of low cancer risk. While getting regular mammograms lowers one’s risk of dying from breast cancer, it does not eliminate one’s risk of getting breast cancer. It is possible that women in our sample misperceived the act of engaging in preventive care, such as obtaining a mammogram, would reduce their risk of developing breast cancer. However, prior screening history was not significantly associated with any of the three measures of perceived risk in our sample. These findings support that the relationship between risk perceptions and screening behavior may be less salient among Hispanic women, compared to non-Hispanic White and Black women, and that other factors such as education and prior family history may play a larger role in shaping risk perceptions [30, 34].

Moreover, 92% of women in our sample spoke Spanish as their primary language. It is well documented that Spanish-speaking, Hispanic women experience numerous barriers to care that may overwhelm the motivational impact of perceiving one to be at risk for breast cancer. However, it is important to note that all women in our study were recruited at the time of a mammography screening appointment suggesting that they were able to overcome barriers to access and use. To this end, the low levels of perceived risk we observed may reflect low levels of knowledge, awareness, or communication around a woman’s personal risk for breast cancer. Improving knowledge and awareness of breast cancer risk factors is central to risk communication and should be considered alongside beliefs and values around risk factors. The term “risk” may also hold different meaning for Spanish-speaking women compared to experts and other groups [35]. More research is needed to understand Hispanic women’s knowledge of risk factors and how these factors shape risk perceptions. These efforts should also be considered alongside complex social and structural influences (i.e., access to care, cost, transportation, literacy) that have shown to hinder uptake of breast cancer preventive care and drive existing disparities in breast cancer outcomes [23, 36].

Prior studies among Hispanic women found that family history was not sufficient to increase breast cancer risk perceptions [30, 37]. However, we found that primary family history was associated with higher levels of perceived risk across all three measures among Hispanic women under the age of 50 and associated with a > 10% lifetime risk and at least moderately high ordinal risk among Hispanic women aged 50 and older. Yet, breast cancer resulting from familial predisposition is thought to account for only 15 to 25% of all diagnosed cases [38, 39]. Overreliance on family history of breast cancer to determine one’s own breast cancer risk may skew not only breast cancer risk perception, but may also affect rates of repeat mammography screening [40]. Additionally, we observed differences in at least two of the measures of perceived risk by language among women of both cohorts. This could reflect how women think about the term “risk.” Risk and the beliefs about the causes, curability, and risk factors of breast cancer differ for lay women compared to health care experts [41], and this difference may be more pronounced in non-English speaking populations. Thus, more research is needed to improve our understanding of how Spanish-speaking, Hispanic women understand and appraise their risk to inform communication strategies that are essential for enhancing implementation of risk-based screening approaches.

Strengths and Limitations

The cross-sectional nature of our study design limits causal interpretation since all measures were collected at the same time. All participants were also recruited from a single FQHC clinic during the time of a mammogram appointment limiting generalizability and possibly biasing our findings. Given that all participants were receiving preventive care, it is possible that they believed receipt of preventive care lowers one’s breast cancer risk. It is also possible that low levels of risk observed in our population were appropriate. A study by our team found that 6.8% of women in our sample had an estimated Gail Model risk > 10%, but nearly 50% of women with an estimated risk > 10% reported their perceived lifetime risk to be less than 10% [42]. However, the homogenous nature of our sample accessing screening services provides critical insights into how similar populations think about their breast cancer risk and additional factors potentially contributing to low levels of risk (poor provider communication, lack of knowledge). As previously mentioned, we observed inconsistencies in how women in our sample responded to three common measures of perceived risk, suggesting that these measures may lack cultural resonance. These inconsistencies, combined with the decision to dichotomize percent lifetime risk, may have limited our ability to understand factors associated with one’s perceived risk for breast cancer.

Conclusion

Improving risk communication and perceptions, particularly among those at high risk, is crucial for implementing risk-based screening and risk reduction strategies. Our findings suggest that the perceived risk for breast cancer, as conceptualized in common measures by prevention science and health behavior theory, may lack cultural resonance or play a less important role in screening behavior. Future studies are needed to understand how underserved, Spanish-speaking, Hispanic women think about and formulate their breast cancer risk to inform the development of strategies to improve risk communication in a manner that is culturally and linguistically appropriate. Potential strategies may include the use of community health workers or Promotoras to deliver education around breast cancer risk and risk-reduction strategies and efforts to improve provider knowledge, assessment, and communication skills around breast cancer risk. However, an unintended consequence of informing women about their risk is an increase in worry or concern [43, 44] and the inability to provide high-risk women receiving care in FQHC with guideline recommended supplemental screening or chemoprevention, which may cause more distress and exacerbate disparities. These factors should be considered as we continue to shift from age-based to more risk-based approaches to breast cancer screening and treatment.