Skip to main content
Log in

Does Doodling Effect Performance: Comparison Across Retrieval Strategies

  • Research in Progress
  • Published:
Psychological Studies Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

Day dreaming and mind wandering are common phenomena that use mental energy and can be distracting. A common strategy employed against day dreaming is doodling (simple unfocused drawing made while a person’s attention is otherwise occupied). Jackie Andrade (2009) reported that, doodling help people manage distractions which in turn leads to improved performance on memory tasks. The present study tries to evaluate the hypothesis whether the benefits of doodling vary across retrieval strategies (recall vs recognition). In the present experiment two groups (retrieval strategy: recall vs recognition) of participants (32 = 16 + 16) with mean age 19.8 ± 2 yrs were involved in a doodling task while a boring story was played in the background. This was followed retrieval tests of information embedded in the audio story. One way analysis of variance (dependent: memory accuracy, independent: recall vs recognition) reported main effects of retrieval strategy (F 1, 30 = 3.428, p = 0.07, Mean recognition = 4.625 ± 2.5, Mean recall = 3.125 ± 1.4). Pearson correlation between doodling percentage and memory accuracy reported r = −0.05, p = 0.77). The results Doodling benefits memory retrieval using recognition over recall. However this benefit is independent of amount of doodling.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Subscribe and save

Springer+ Basic
$34.99 /Month
  • Get 10 units per month
  • Download Article/Chapter or eBook
  • 1 Unit = 1 Article or 1 Chapter
  • Cancel anytime
Subscribe now

Buy Now

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Fig. 1
Fig. 2

Similar content being viewed by others

References

  • Andrade, J. (2009). What does doodling do? Applied Cognitive Psychology, 24, 100–106.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Anderson, J., & Bower, G. (1972). Recognition and retrieval process in free recall. Psychological Review, 79, 97–123.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bahrick, H., & Bahrick, P. (1964). A re-examination of the inter-relations among measures of retention. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 16, 318–324.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Brown, J. (1965). A comparison of recognition and recall by a multiple response method. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 4, 401–408.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Cabeza, R., Kapur, S., Craik, F., McIntosh, A., Houle, S., & Tulving, E. (1997). Functional neuroanatomy of recall and recognition: A PET study of episodic memory. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 9, 254–265.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • David, R., Sutherland, N., & Judd, R. (1961). Information content in recognition and recall. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 61, 422–429.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Do, S., & Schallert, D. (2004). Emotions and classroom talk: Toward a model of the role of affect in students’ experiences of classroom discussions. Journal of Educational Psychology, 96, 619–634.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Gillund, G., & Shiffrin, R. (1984). A retrieval model for both recognition and recall. Psychological Review, 91, 1–67.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Hickson, S., & Reed, W. (2009). Do constructed-response and multiple-choice questions measure the same thing? University of Canterbury, New Zealand: Department of Economics & Finance. Working Paper No. 08/2009.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kintsch, W. (1970). Models of free recall and recognition. In D. A. Norman (Ed.), Models of human memory. New York: Academic.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kintsch, W. (1974). The representation of meaning in memory. New York: Lawrence Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Klinger, E. (1971). Structure and Functions of Fantasy. New York: Wiley.

    Google Scholar 

  • McNulty, J. (1965). An analysis of recall and recognition process in verbal learning. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 4(5), 430–436.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Muter, P. (1978). Recognition failure of recallable words in semantic memory. Memory and Cognition, 6(1), 9–12.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Postaman, L. (1975). Tests of generality of the principles of encoding specificity. Memory & Cognition, 3(6), 663–672.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Reder, L., Anderson, J., & Bjork, R. (1974). A semantic interpretation of encoding specificity. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 102, 648–656.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Schot, G. (2011). Doodling and the default network of the brain. The Lancet, 378, 1133.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Singer, J. (1996). Daydreaming. New York: Plenum Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Smallwood, J., O’Connor, R., Sudbury, M., & Obonsavin, M. (2007a). Mind wandering and Dysphoria. Cognition and Emotion, 21, 816–842.

  • Smallwood, J., Fishman, D., & Shooler, J. (2007b). Counting the cost of absent mind: mind wandering as an unrecognized influence on educational performance. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 14, 230–236.

  • Smallwood, J., & Schooler, J. (2006). The restless mind. Psychological Bulletin, 132, 946–958.

  • Teasadale, J., Proctor, L., Lloyds, L., & Baddeley, A. (1993). Working memory and stimulus independent thought: effects of memory load and presentation rate. European Journal of Cognitive Psychology, 5, 417–433.

  • Tulving, E. (1968). When recall is higher than recognition? Psychonomic Science, 10(2), 53–54.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Tulving, E. (1972). Episodic and semantic memory. In E. Tulving & W. Donaldson (Eds.), Organization of Memory. New York: Academic.

    Google Scholar 

  • Tulving, E., & Thompson, D. (1973). Encoding specificity and retrieval processes in episodic memory. Psychological Review, 80, 352–373.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Tulving, E. (1976). Ecphoric processes in recall and recognition. In J. Brown (Ed.), Recall and Recognition. London: John Wiley & Sons.

    Google Scholar 

  • Walker, I., & Hulme, C. (1999). Concrete words are easier to recall that abstract words: Evidence for a semantic contribution to short term serial recall. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning Memory and Cognition, 22(1), 55–62.

    Google Scholar 

  • Watkins, M., & Tulving, E. (1975). Episodic memory: When recognition fails. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 104, 5–29.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Wilson, K., & Korn, J. (2007). Attention during lectures: Beyond ten minutes. Teaching of Psychology, 34, 85–89.

    Article  Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to N. Kashyap.

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Singh, T., Kashyap, N. Does Doodling Effect Performance: Comparison Across Retrieval Strategies. Psychol Stud 60, 7–11 (2015). https://doi.org/10.1007/s12646-014-0293-3

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s12646-014-0293-3

Keywords