Skip to main content
Log in

Theory of Mind Deficit in Schizophrenia and Associated Cognitive Functions

  • Assessment
  • Published:
Psychological Studies Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

This study examined the Theory of Mind (ToM) ability of schizophrenic persons in association with other cognitive functions like memory, executive functioning and attention. Results showed evidence of relationship between ToM and cognitive functions i.e. executive function, working memory and intelligence but no uniform predictive model was found across all the ToM tasks. It was also noted that predicting variables related to different cognitive domains varied markedly for schizophrenia and control groups. It seems that there are multiple cognitive pathways to ToM mechanism, although these different forms may not have an additive effect.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Similar content being viewed by others

References

  • Baddley, A. D. (1986). Working memory. New York: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Baron-Cohen, S., Leslie, A., & Frith, U. (1985). Does the autistic child have a ‘theory of mind’? Cognition, 21, 37–46.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Bommer, I., & Brune, M. (2006). Social cognition in pure delusional disorder. Cognitive Neuropsychiatry, 11, 493–503.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Bora, E., Eryavuz, A., & Kayahan, B. (2006). Social functioning, theory of mind and neurocognition in outpatients with schizophrenia; mental state decoding may be a better predictor of social functioning than mental state reasoning. Psychiatry Research, 145, 95–103.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Bora, E., Sehitoglu, G., Aslier, M., & Atabay, I. (2007). Theory of mind and unawareness of illness in schizophrenia: Is poor insight a mentalizing deficit? European Archives of Psychiatry and Clinical Neuroscience, 257, 104–111.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Bora, E., Yucel, M., & Pantelis, C. (2009). Theory of mind impairement in schizophrenia: Meta –analysis. Schizophrenia Research, 109(1–3), 1–9.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Botvinick, M., Nystrom, L., Fissell, E., Carter, C. S., & Cohen, J. D. (1999). Conflict monitoring versus selection for action in interior cingulated cortex. Nature, 402, 179–181.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Braver, T. S., Barch, D. M., Gray, J. R., Molfese, D. L., & Synder, A. (2001). Anterior cingulated cortex and response conflict: Effects of frequency, inhibition and errors. Cerebral Cortex, 11, 825–836.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Brothers, L. (1990). The social brain: A project for integrating primate behavior and neurophysiology in a new domain. Concepts Neuroscience, 1, 27–51.

    Google Scholar 

  • Brune, M. (2001). Social cognition and psychopathology in an evolutionary perspective-current status and proposal for research. Psychopathology, 34, 85–94.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Brune, M. (2003). Theory of mind and the role of IQ in the chronic disorganized schizophrenia. Schizophrenia Research, 60, 57–64.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Brüne, M. (2005). Emotion recognition, “theory of mind,” and social behavior in schizophrenia. Psychiatry Research, 133(2–3).

  • Brune, M. (2005). “Theory of mind” in schizophrenia: A review of the literature. Schizophrenia Bulletin, 31(1), 21–42.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Brunet, E., Sarfati, Y., & Hardy-Bayle, M. C. (2003). Abnormalities of brain function during a nonverbal theory of mind task in schizophrenia. Neuropsychologia, 41, 1574–1582.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Collette, F., Van der Linden, M., Laureys, S., et al. (2005). Exploring the unity and diversity of the neural substrates of executive functioning. Human Brain Mapping, 25(4), 409–423.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Corcoran, R., Mercer, G., & Frith, C. D. (1995). Schizophrenia symptomatology and social inference: Investigating ‘theory of mind’ in people with schizophrenia. Schizophrenia Research, 17, 5–13.

    Google Scholar 

  • Corcoran, R., & Frith, C. D. (1996). Conversational conduct and symptoms of schizophrenia. Cognitive Neuropsychiatry, 1, 305–318.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Davis, H. L., & Pratt, C. (1996). The development of children’s theory of mind: The working memory of explanation. Australian Journal of Psychology, 47, 25–31.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Fisk, J. E., & Sharp, C. A. (2004). Age-related impairment in executive functioning: Updating, inhibition, shifting, and access. Journal of Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology, 26(7), 874–890.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Frith, C. D. (1994). Theory of mind in schizophrenia. In J. C. Cutting & A. S. David (Eds.), The neuropsychology of schizophrenia. Hove: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.

    Google Scholar 

  • Frith, C. D., & Corcoran, R. (1996). Exploring theory of mind in people with schizophrenia. Psychological Medicine, 26, 521–530.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Gallagher, H. L., Happe, F., Brunswick, N., Fletcher, P. G., Frith, U., & Frith, C. D. (2000). Reading the mind in cartoons and stories: An FMRI study of theory of mind in verbal and non verbal tasks. Neuropsychology, 38, 11–21.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Gordon, A. C. L., & Olson, D. R. (1998). The relation between acquisition of a theory of mind and the capacity to hold in mind. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 68, 70–83.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Green, M. F. (1996). What are the functional consequences of neurocognitive deficits in schizophrenia? The American Journal of Psychiatry, 153, 321–330.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Green, M. F., & Nuechterlein, K. H. (1999). Should schizophrenia be treated as a neurocognitive disorder. Schizophrenia Bulletin, 25, 309–318.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Hardy-Bayle, M. C., Sarfati, Y., Olivier, V., Besche, C., Lefrere, F., Passerieux, C. (1996). Conscience Intentionnelle et Schizophrenie, 3, 104–107. As cited in Sarfati, Y., Hardy-Bayle, M.C., Nadel, J., Chevalier, J.F., & Widlocher, D. (1997b). Attribution of mental states of others in schizophrenic patients. Cognitive Neuropsychiatry, 2, 1–17.

  • Heaton, R. K. (1981). Wisconsin card sorting test manual. Odessa: Psychological Assessment Resources.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hughes, C. (1998). Executive function in preschoolers. Links with theory of mind and verbal ability. British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 16, 233–253.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Inc, S. P. S. S. (2000). SPSS for windows release 11. Chicago: Standard Edition.

    Google Scholar 

  • Janssen, I., Krabbendam, L., Jolles, J., & Os, J. V. (2003). Alterations in theory of mind in patients with schizophrenia and non- psychotic relatives. Acta Psychiatrica Scandinavia, 108, 110–117.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Jha, M. (2010). An exploration of theory of mind in schizophrenic subjects. Indian Journal of Clinical Psychology, 37(2), 88–100.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kay, S. R., Fisz-Bein, A., & Opler, L. A. (1987). The positive and negative syndrome scale (PANSS) for schizophrenia. Schizophrenia Bulletin, 13, 261–274.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Keck, P. E., Jr., McElroy, S. L., & Strakowski, S. M. (1998). 12 months outcome of patients with bipolar disorders following hospitalization for a manic or mixed episode. The American Journal of Psychiatry, 155, 646–652.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Kosmidis, M. H., Giannakou, M., Giorgos, G., et al. (2011). The impact of impaired “theory of mind” on social intraction in schizophrenia. Journal of International Neuropsychological Society, 17, 511–521.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Langdon, R., & Coltheart, M. (2001). Mentalising, executive planning and disengagement in schizophrenia. Cognitive Neuropsychiatry, 6, 81–108.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Langdon, R., Michie, P., Ward, P. B., et al. (1997). Defective self and/or other mentalising in schizophrenia: A cognitive neuropsychological approach. Cognitive Neuropychiatry, 2, 167–193.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Langdon, R., Ward, P. B., & Coltheart, M. (2008). Reasonong anamolies associated with delusion in schizophrenia. Schizophrenia Bulletin, 11, 367–384.

    Google Scholar 

  • Leslie, A. M., & Frith, U. (1988). Autistic children’s understanding of seeing, knowing and believing. British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 6, 315–324.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lough, S., Gregory, C., & Hodges, J. R. (2001). Dissociation of social cognition and executive function in frontal variant frontotemporal demential. Neurocase, 7, 123–130.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Mandal, M. K., Panday, G., Singh, S. K., & Asthana, H. S. (1992). Hand preferences in India. Indian Journal of Psychiatry, 27, 433–442.

    Google Scholar 

  • Mitcheley, N. J., Barber, J., Gray, J. M., Brooks, N., & Livingston, M. G. (1998). Comprehension of irony in schizophrenia. Cognitive Neuropsychiatry, 3(2), 127–138.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Miyake, A., Friedman, N. P., Emerson, M. J., et al. (2000). The unity and diversity of executive functions and their contributions to complex ‘frontal lobe’ tasks: A latent variable analysis. Cognitive Psychology, 41(1), 49–100.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Murphy, D. (1998). Theory of mind in a sample of men with schizophrenia detained in a special hospital: Its relationship to symptom profiles and neuropsychological tests. Crime Behaviour Mental Health, 8, 13–26.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Narr, K. L., Thompson, P. M., & Sharma, T. (2001). A three-dimensional mapping of gyral shape and cortical surface asymmetries in schizophrenia. Gender effects. The American Journal of Psychiatry, 158, 244–255.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Perner, J., & Lang, B. (2001). Theory of mind and executive function: Cognitive complexity or functional dependence? In P. D. Zelazo & J. W. Astington (Eds.), Developing theories if intentions: Social understanding and self control. NJ: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Pershad, D., & Verma, S. K. (1988). PGI battery of brain dysfunction. Agra: National Psychological Corporation.

    Google Scholar 

  • Pickup, G. J. (2008). Relationship between theory of mind and executive function in schizophrenia: A systematic review. Psychopathology, 41(4), 206–213.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Pickup, G. J., & Frith, C. D. (2001). Theory of mind impairments in schizophrenia: Symptomatology, severity and specificity. Psychological Medicine, 31, 207–220.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Ramalingaswamy, P. (1974). Manual of Indian adaptation of WAIS–performance scale. Delhi: Manasayan.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rao, S. L., Subbakrishna, D. K., Gopukumar, K. (2004). NIMHANS neuropsychology battery–2004 manual, NIMHANS, NIMHANS Publication No. 60.

  • Riley, E. M., McGovern, D., & Mockler, D. (1999). Neuropsychological functioning in first- episode psychosis- evidence of specific deficits. Schizophrenia Research, 43, 47–55.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Rowe, A. D., Bullock, P. R., Polkey, C. E., & Morris, R. G. (2001). ‘Theory of mind’ impairments and their relationship to executive functioning following frontal lobe excision. Brain, 124, 600–616.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Sarfati, Y., Hardy-Baylé, M. C., & Nadel, J. (1997). Attribution of mental states to others in schizophrenic patients. Cognitive Neuropsychiatry, 2, 1–17.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Sarfati, Y., Hardy Bayle, M. C., Besche, C., & Widlocher, D. (1997). Attributions of intentions to others in people with schizophrenia: A non-verbal exploration with comic-strip. Schizophrenia Research, 25, 199–209.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Sarfati, Y., Hardy-Bayle, M. C., Brunet, E., & Widlocher, D. (1999). Investigating theory of mind in schizophrenia: Influence of verbalization in disorganized and non-disorganized patients. Schizophrenia Research, 37, 183–190.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Saxe, R., Schulz, L. E., & Jiang, Y. V. (2006). Reading minds versus following rules: Dissociating theory of mind and executive control in the brain. Society for Neuroscience, 1(3–4), 284–298.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Shallice, T. (1982). Specific impairment of planning. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society of London, 298, 199–209.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Shamsunder, C., Shanmugam, V., Sriram, T. J., & Murliraj, S. G. (1986). Validity of short 5-item version of the General Health Questionnaire (G.H.Q.). Indian Journal of Psychiatry, 28, 217–219.

    Google Scholar 

  • Sharma, T., & Antonova, L. (2003). Cognitive functions in schizophrenia. Deficits, functional consequences and future treatment. Psychiatric Clinics of North America, 26, 25–40.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Shelton, J. T., Elliott, E. M., Hill, B. D., et al. (2009). A comparison of laboratory and clinical working memory tests and their prediction of fluid intelligence. Intelligence, 37(3), 283–294.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Spreen, O., & Strauss, J. (1998). A compendium of neuropsychological tests. Administration, norms and commentary (2nd ed.). New York: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Sprong, M., Schothorst, P., & Vos, E. (2007). Theory of mind in schizophrenia: Meta analysis. The British Journal of Psychiatry, 191, 5–13.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Stroop, J. R. (1935). Studies on interference in serial verbal reaction. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 18, 643–662.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Wechsler, D. (1997). Wechsler adult intelligence scale (3rd ed.). San Antonio: The Psychological Corporation.

    Google Scholar 

  • Whiten, A. (2000). Social complexity and social intelligence. Neurotic Foundation Symposium, 233, 185–196.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Wimmer, H., & Perner, J. (1983). Belief about beliefs: Representation and the constraining function of wrong beliefs in young childrens’ understanding of deception. Cognitive, 13, 103–128.

    Google Scholar 

  • World Health Organization. (1993). The ICD-10 classification of mental and behavioural disorders: Diagnostic criteria for research. Geneva: W H O.

    Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgments

I express my gratitude to late Dr. L.M. Sahu for supervising this research work.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Manisha Jha.

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Cite this article

Jha, M. Theory of Mind Deficit in Schizophrenia and Associated Cognitive Functions. Psychol Stud 57, 283–291 (2012). https://doi.org/10.1007/s12646-012-0148-8

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s12646-012-0148-8

Keywords

Navigation