Skip to main content
Log in

Gulliver’s Eggs: Why Methods are not an Issue of Qualitative Research in Cultural Psychology

  • Regular Article
  • Published:
Integrative Psychological and Behavioral Science Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

The future of qualitative methods regards the kind of object cultural psychology is interested and the kind of questions it can ask. I propose that the object should be experiencing, understood as a complex whole, consisting of lived-by action and counter-action, that is contextual inter-action with the world in the form of an experiencing subject and otherness. The kind of questions cultural psychology can ask is instead related to the epistemological status attributed to both researcher and participant. Probably few scholars such as Vygotsky, Piaget and Lewin understood to what extent experiencing is always changing, because the relationship between mind, alterity and culture is co-generative. This also implies a relativization and a decentralization of the psychology’s perspective. Finally, I provide some examples from the history of psychology and some suggestions to work at the level of such complexity by using methods that can work with complex objects such as products of human activity (e.g., art, literature, architecture, etc.).

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Fig. 1
Fig. 2

Similar content being viewed by others

Notes

  1. The term “state” could sound a bit old-fashioned. Nevertheless, only in year 2013, PsychINFO Database reports 342 original peer-reviewed journal articles using this term in the abstract. It is used of course with different meanings in very each area of psychology (e.g., Schizophrenia & Psychotic States; Health & Mental Health Services; Promotion & Maintenance of Health & Wellness; Educational Administration & Personnel; Social Processes & Social Issues; Professional Personnel Attitudes & Characteristics; Curriculum & Programs & Teaching Methods; Professional Education & Training; Classroom Dynamics & Student Adjustment & Attitudes; Behavior Disorders & Antisocial Behavior; Community & Social Services). Even though this is just a superficial observation, it tells us that the term is still in wide use.

  2. Subject’s reaction time in seconds.

  3. This is probably the first historical mention of the so-called aha-experience.

References

  • Allport, G.W. (1967). Gordon W. Allport. In E. G. Boring, and G. Lindzey (Eds.), A history of psychology in autobiography, Vol V, (pp. 1–25). East Norwalk: Appleton-Century-Crofts. doi:10.1037/11579-000

  • Bakhtin, M.M. (1996). Forms of time and of the chronotope in the novel. In M.M. Bakhtin, The dialogic imagination: Four essays (Michael Holquist Ed) (pp. 84–258). Austin: University of Texas Press.

  • Bhabha, H. K. (1990). DissemiNation: time, narrative, and the margins of the modern nation. In H. K. Bhabha (Ed.), Nation and narration (pp. 291–322). New York: Routledge.

    Google Scholar 

  • Brinkmann, S. (2007). Could interviews be epistemic?: an alternative to qualitative opinion polling. Qualitative Inquiry, 13(8), 1116–1138. doi:10.1177/1077800407308222.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Buehler, K. (1951). On thought connections. In D. Rapaport (Ed.), Organization and pathology of thought. Selected sources (pp. 39–57). New York: Columbia University Press.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Creswell, J. W. (2009). Research design: qualitative, quantitative, and mixed methods approaches. Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage Publications.

    Google Scholar 

  • Davidson, D. (1980). Mental events. In D. Davidson (Ed.), Essays on action and events (pp. 207–227). Oxford: Claredon Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Garfinkel, H. (2002). Ethnomethodology’s program. New York: Rowan and Littlefield.

    Google Scholar 

  • Herbst, D. P. (1995). What happens when we make a distinction: An elementary introduction to cogenetic logic. In T. Kindermann & J. Valsiner (Eds.), Development of person-context relations (pp. 67–79). Hillsdale: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Höffding, H. (1905). The present state of psychology and its relations to the neighboring sciences. Psychological Review, XII(2–3), 67–77.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • James, W. (1950). The principles of psychology (Vol. 1). Mineola: Dover Publications.

    Google Scholar 

  • Liberman, K. (2013). More studies in ethnomethodology. Albany: State University of New York Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Merrell, F. (2005). Capoeira and Candomblé: conformity and resistance through afro-Brazilian experience. Princeton: Marcus Wiener Publishers.

    Google Scholar 

  • Michell, J. (2010). The quantity/quality interchange: a blind spot on the highway of science. In A. Toomela & J. Valsiner (Eds.), Methodological thinking in psychology: 60 years gone astray? (pp. 45–68). Charlotte: Information Age Publishing.

    Google Scholar 

  • Morey, L. C. (1987). Observations on the meeting between Allport and Freud. Psychoanalytic Review, 74(1), 135–139.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Mörtl, K., & Gelo, O.C.G. (2015). Qualitative methods in psychotherapy process research. In Psychotherapy research (pp. 381–428). Springer Vienna.

  • Peirce, C. S. (1935). In: C. Hartshorne, P. Weiss (Eds) Collected papers of Charles Sanders Peirce, vols 1–6. Cambridge: Harvard University Press [original in 1901].

  • Rommetveit, R. (1992). Outlines of a dialogically based social-cognitive approach to human cognition and communication. In A. H. Wold (Ed.), The dialogical alternative (pp. 19–44). Oslo: Scandinavian University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Salvatore, S. (2011). Psychotherapy research needs theory. Outline for an epistemology of the clinical exchange. Integrative Psychological and Behavioral Science, 45(3), 366–388. doi:10.1007/s12124-011-9180-9.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Salvatore, S., Gennaro, A., Valsiner, J., & Valsiner, J. (Eds.). (2013). Making sense of infinite uniqueness. The emerging system of idiographic science (Yearbook of idiographic science 4th ed.). Charlotte: IAP, Information Age Publishing.

    Google Scholar 

  • Sellars, W. (1963). Science, perception and reality. London: Routledge & Kegan Paul Ltd.

    Google Scholar 

  • Starks, H., & Trinidad, S. B. (2007). Choose your method: a comparison of phenomenology, discourse analysis, and grounded theory. Qualitative Health Research, 17(10), 1372–1380. doi:10.1177/1049732307307031.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Swift, J. (2005). Gulliver’s travels. In P. M. Parker (Ed.), Webster’s thesaurus edition. San Diego: ICON Group International.

    Google Scholar 

  • Tateo, L. (2013). Generalization as creative and reflective act: revisiting Lewin’s conflict between Aristotelian and Galileian modes of thought in psychology. Theory & Psychology, 23(4), 518–536. doi:10.1177/0959354313488844.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Tateo, L. (2014). The dialogical dance: self, identity construction, positioning and embodiment in tango dancers. Integrative Psychological & Behavioral Science, 48(3), 299–321. doi:10.1007/s12124-014-9258-2.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Tateo, L. (2015a). Temporality and generalization in psychology: time as context. In L. M. Simão, D. S. Guimarães, & J. Valsiner (Eds.), Temporality: culture in the flow of human experience. Charlotte: IAP, Information Age Publishers.

    Google Scholar 

  • Tateo, L. (2015b). Giambattista Vico and the principles of cultural psychology: A programmatic retrospective. History of Human Sciences.

  • Tateo, L., & Marsico, G. (2013). The self as tension of wholeness and emptiness. Interacções, 24, 1–19.

    Google Scholar 

  • Tateo, L., & Marsico, G. (2014). Open complementarity in cultural psychology. In B. Wagoner, N. Chaudhary, & P. Hviid (Eds.), Culture psychology and Its future: complementarity in a New Key (pp. 77–91). Charlotte: IAP, Information Age Publications.

    Google Scholar 

  • Valsiner, J. (1998). The guided mind: a sociogenetic approach to personality. Cambridge: Harvard University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Valsiner, J. (2012). A guided science: history of psychology in the mirror of its making. New Brunswick: Transaction Publishers.

    Google Scholar 

  • Valsiner, J. (2014a). An invitation to cultural psychology. London: Sage.

    Google Scholar 

  • Valsiner, J. (2014b). Functional reality of the quasi-real: Gegenstandstheorie and cultural psychology today. Culture & Psychology, 20(3), 285–307. doi:10.1177/1354067X14542532.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Varzi, A. (2001). The best question. Journal of Philosophical Logic, 30(3), 251–258. doi:10.1023/A:1017549407042.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Vygotsky, L. S. (1978). Interaction between learning and development. In M. Cole, V. John-Steiner, & E. Souberman (Eds.), Mind in society: the development of higher psychological processes (pp. 79–91). Cambridge: Harvard University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wiley, N. (1994). The semiotic self. Cambridge: Polity Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wright Mills, C. (1959). The sociological imagination. New York: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgments

This work has been funded by the Marie Curie IEF-2012 grant “EPICS. Epistemology in psychological science, the heritage of Giambattista Vico and the cultural psychology” at Aalborg University (Denmark).

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Luca Tateo.

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Tateo, L. Gulliver’s Eggs: Why Methods are not an Issue of Qualitative Research in Cultural Psychology. Integr. psych. behav. 49, 187–201 (2015). https://doi.org/10.1007/s12124-015-9296-4

Download citation

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s12124-015-9296-4

Keywords

Navigation