Skip to main content
Log in

Consciousness, context, and know-how

  • Published:
Synthese Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

In this paper I criticize the most significant recent examples of the practical knowledge analysis of knowledge-how in the philosophical literature: David Carr [1979, Mind, 88, 394–409; 1981a, American Philosophical Quarterly, 18, 53–61; 1981b, Journal of Philosophy of Education, 15(1), 87–96] and Stanley & Williamson [2001, Journal of Philosophy, 98(8), 411–444]. I stress the importance of know-how in our contemporary understanding of the mind, and offer the beginnings of a treatment of know-how capable of providing insight in to the use of know-how in contemporary cognitive science. Specifically, I claim that Carr’s necessary conditions for know-how fail to capture the distinction he himself draws between ability and knowing-how. Moreover, Carr ties knowing-how to conscious intent, and to an explicit knowledge of procedural rules. I argue that both moves are mistakes, which together render Carr’s theory an inadequate account both of common ascriptions of knowledge-how and of widely accepted ascriptions of knowledge-how within explanations in cognitive science. Finally, I note that Carr’s conditions fail to capture intuitions (heshares) regarding the ascription of know-how to persons lacking ability. I then consider the position advocated by Stanley & Williamson (2001), which seems avoid Carr’s commitments to conscious intent and explicit knowledge while still maintaining that “knowledge-how is simply a species of knowledge-that" (Stanley & Williamson, 2001, p. 411). I argue that Stanley and Williamson’s attempt to frame a reductionist view that avoids consciously occurrent beliefs during exercises of knowledge-how and explicit knowledge of procedural rules is both empirically implausible and explanatorily vacuous. In criticizing these theories I challenge the presuppositions of the most pervasive response to Ryle in the philosophic literature, what might be described as “the received view." I also establish several facts about knowing-how. First, neither conscious intent nor explicit representation (much less conscious representation) of procedural rules are necessary for knowing-how given the theory of cognition current in cognitive science. I argue that the discussed analyses fail to capture the necessary conditions for knowledge-how because know-how requires the instantiation of an ability and of the capacities necessary for exploiting an ability—not conscious awareness of purpose or explicit knowledge of rules. Second, one must understand knowledge-how as task-specific, i.e., as presupposing certain underlying conditions. Conceiving of know-how as task-specific allows one to understand ascriptions of know-how in the absence of ability as counterfactual ascriptions based upon underlying competence.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Similar content being viewed by others

References

  • Anderson J.R., Kline P.J., Beasley C.M. (1979). A general learning theory andits application to schema abstraction. In: G.H. Bower (eds) The psychology of learning and motivation. NewYork, Academic Press

    Google Scholar 

  • Annett J. (1995). On knowing how to do things: A theory of motor imagery. Cognitive Brain Research 3, 65–69

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • The American Psychiatric Association (1987). Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders: Third edition-revised. Washington: American Psychiatric Association.

  • Armstrong D.M. (1968). A materialist theory of mind. London, Routledge and Kegan Paul Ltd

    Google Scholar 

  • Bargh J., Chartrand T. (1999). The unbearable automaticity of being. American Psychologist 54, 462–479

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Baseler H.,Morland A., Wandell B. (1999). Topographic organization of human visual areas in the absence of input from primary cortex. Journal of Neuroscience 19, 2619–2627

    Google Scholar 

  • Bayley P., Squire L. (2002). Medial temporal lobe amnesia: Gradual acquisition of factual information by nondeclarative memory. The Journal of Neuroscience 22(13): 5741–5748

    Google Scholar 

  • Bechtel W., Abrahamsen A. (1991). Connectionism and the mind: An introduction to parallel processing in networks. Cambridge, Blackwell Press

    Google Scholar 

  • Behrmann M., Marotta J., Gauthier I., Tarr M., McKeeff T. (2005). Behavioral change and its neural correlates invisual agnosia after expertise Training. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience 17, 554–568

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Berry D., Broadbent D. (1984). On the relationship between task performance and associated verbalizable knowledge. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology A: Human Experimental Psychology 36A(2): 209–231

    Google Scholar 

  • Bliss, J., Ogborn, J. (1994). Force and motion from the beginning. Learning & Instruction, 4(1), 7–25 (tacit).

    Google Scholar 

  • Brehmer, B.,Hagafors, R., & Johansson, R. (1980). Cognitive skills in judgment: subjects’ ability to use information about weights, function forms, and organizing principles. Organizational Behavior and Human Performance, 26(3), 373–385 (Expert judgement).

    Google Scholar 

  • Brown D. (1970). Knowing how and knowing that, what. In: Wood O.P., Pitcher G. (eds) Ryle. Garden City, Anchor Books

    Google Scholar 

  • Bull R. (1979). Pupil dilation, sex of stimulus, and age and sex of observer. Perceptual and Motor Skills 49(1): 27–30

    Google Scholar 

  • Carr D.(1979). The logic of knowing how and ability. Mind 88, 394–409

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Carr D. (1981a). Knowledge in practice. American Philosophical Quarterly 18, 53–61

    Google Scholar 

  • Carr D. (1981b). On mastering a skill. Journal of Philosophy of Education 15(1): 87–96

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Cermak L.S., Lewis R., Butters N., Goodglass H.(1973). Role of verbal mediation in performance of motor tasks by Korsak offpatients. Perceptual and Motor Skills 37, 259–262

    Google Scholar 

  • Chervinskaya K., Bekhterev V.(2000). Some methodological aspects of tacit knowledge elicitation. The Journal of Experimental & Theoretical Artificial Intelligence 12(1): 43–55

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Cohen J.,Eichenbaum H., Deacedo B., Corkin S. (1985). Different memory systems underlying acquisition of procedural and declarative knowledge. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences 444, 54–71

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Cohen J.,Hunt C., Romine L., Wszalek T., Nash C.(1999). Hippocampal system and declarative (relational) memory: Summarizingthe data from functionalneuroimaging studies. Hippocampus 9, 83–99

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Cohen J., Squire L. (1980). Preserved learning and retention of pattern–analyzing skill in amnesia: Dissociation of knowing how and knowing that. Science 210, 207–209

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Cooke N. (1999). Knowledge elicitation. In: Durso F. (ed) Handbook of applied cognition. NewYork, NY John Wiley and Sons, pp. 479-509

    Google Scholar 

  • Corkin S. (1968). Acquisition of motor skill after bilateral medial temporal–lobe excision. Neuro psychologia 6(3): 255–266

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Cummins R.(1983). The nature of psychological explanation. Cambridge, MITPress

    Google Scholar 

  • Dawes R., Faust D., Meehl P. (1989). Clinical versus actuarial judgment. Science 243(4899): 1668–1674

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • de Groot A., Gobet F. (1996). Perception and memory inchess. Heuristics of the professional eye. Assen, VanGorcum

    Google Scholar 

  • Doya K. (2000). Complementary roles of basal Ganglia and cerebellum in learning and motor Control." Current Opinionin Neurobiology 10(6): 732–739

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ellis N. (2005). At the interface: Dynamic interactions of explicit and implicit language knowledge. Studies in second language acquisition 27(2): 305–352

    Google Scholar 

  • Emery N., Clayton N. (2005). Animal cognition. In: Bolhuis J.J., Giraldeau L.-A. (eds) The behavior of animals: Mechanisms, function, and evolution. Malden, MA, Blackwell Publishing

    Google Scholar 

  • Evans, J. (1988).The knowledge elicitation problem: A psychological perspective. Behavior and Information Technology,7(2), pp. 111–130.

    Google Scholar 

  • Faust D. (1989). Data integration in legal evaluations: Can clinicians deliver on their premises?. Behavioral Sciences andthe Law 7(4): 469–483

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ford D., Bergen N., Sterman J. (1998). Expert knowledge elicitation to improve formal and mental models. System Dynamics Review 14(4): 309–340

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Gabrieli J. (1998). Cognitive neuroscience of human memory. Annual Review of Psychology 49, 87–115

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Gabrieli J., McGlinchey–Berroth R., Carrillo M., Gluck M.,Cermak L., Disterhoft J. (1995). Intact delay–eyeblink classical conditioning in amnesia. Behavioral Neuroscience 109(5): 819–827

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ganzach Y. (2001). Nonlinear model of clinical judgement: Communal nonlinearity and nonlinear accuracy. Psychological Science 12(5): 403–407

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Geach P. (1957). Mental acts. NewYork, Humanities Press

    Google Scholar 

  • Geach P. (1966). Dr. Kenny on practical inference. Analysis 26, 76–9

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Gilbert D. (1989).Thinking lightly about others: Automatic components of the social inference process. In: Uleman J.S., Bargh J.A. (eds) Unintended thought. NewYork, Guilford Press, pp. 189-211

    Google Scholar 

  • Goldman, A. (2002). Simulation theory and mental concepts. Advances in Consciousness Research, 45.

  • Gobet F., Simon H.A. (1996). The roles of recognition processes and look-ahead search in time-constrained expertproblem solving: Evidence from grand-master-level chess. PsychologicalScience 6, 381–381

    Google Scholar 

  • Gobet F., Simon H.A. (1998). Expertchess memory:Revisitingthechunking hypothesis. Memory 7, 52–55

    Google Scholar 

  • Grafton S.,Mazziotta J., Presty S., Friston K. (1992). Functional anatomy of human procedural learning determined with regional cerebral blood flow and PET. JournalofNeuroscience 12(7): 2542–2548

    Google Scholar 

  • Griffen D. (1992). Animal minds. Chicago, University of Chicago Press

    Google Scholar 

  • Hansell, M. (1972). Case building behavior of the caddis-fly larvae. (Lepidostoma Hirtum Journal of Zoology) 167, 179–192

  • Hansell M. (1984). Animal architecture and building behavior. London, Longman

    Google Scholar 

  • Haugeland J. (1981). Semantic engines: An introduction to mind design. In: Haugeland J. (ed) Mind design. Cambridge, MIT/Bradford Books

    Google Scholar 

  • Hill, K. (1984). Verbal reports of stimulus effects on learning: Introspection revisited. Journal of Research in Personality, 18(4), 480–490 (Intro).

    Google Scholar 

  • Hintikka J. (1974). Practicalvs. theoretical reason—an ambiguous legacy. In: Korner S. (eds) Practical reason. New Haven, YaleUniversity Press

    Google Scholar 

  • Hoffman, R., Shadbolt, N.,Burton, A., & Klein, G. (1995). Eliciting knowledge from experts: A methodological analysis. Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes, 62(2), 129–158 (Expert).

    Google Scholar 

  • Jacoby L., Witherspoon D. (1982). Remembering without awareness. Canadian Journal of Psychology 36(2): 300–324

    Google Scholar 

  • Kahneman D., Tversky A. (1982). The simulation heuristic. In: Kahneman D., Slovic P., A. Tversky (eds) Judgment under uncertainty: Heuristics and biases. Cambridge, Cambridge University Press

    Google Scholar 

  • Katzoff C. (1984). Knowing How. The Southern Journal of Philosophy 22, 61–70

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kenny A.(1966). Practical inference. In Analysis 26, 65–75

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Knowlton, B., & Squire, L. (1993). The learning of categories: Parallel memory systems for item memory and category–level knowledge. Science 262, 1747–1749 (Kh).

    Google Scholar 

  • Knowlton, B., Squire, L., & Gluck, M.(1994). Probabilistic classification in amnesia. Learning and Memory,1, 106–120 (Kh).

  • Knowlton B.,Mangels J., Squire L.(1996). A neostriatal habit learning systemin humans. Science 273(5282): 1399–1402

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Koethe J. (2002). Stanley and Williamson on knowing how. The Journal of Philosophy 99, 325–328

    Google Scholar 

  • Lovett M.C., Anderson J.R. (2005) Thinking as a production system. In: Holyoak K., Morrison R. (eds)Cambridge handbook of thinking and reasoning. NY, Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Luce G., Segal J. (1966). Sleep. NewYork, Coward McCann

    Google Scholar 

  • Mackie J.(1974). A reply to Jaakko Hintikka. In: Korner S. (eds) Practical reason. New Haven,Yale University Press

    Google Scholar 

  • Mandler G. (1975). Consciousness, respectable, useful, and probably necessary. In: Solso R. (eds) Information processing and cognition: The Loyola syposium. Hillsdale, NJ, Earlbaum

    Google Scholar 

  • Marr D. (1981). Vision. NewYork, Freeman and Company

    Google Scholar 

  • Meehl P. (1954). Clinical vs statistical prediction: A theoretical analysis and are view of the evidence. Minneapolis, MN, University of Minnesota Press

    Google Scholar 

  • Mogenson G., Jones D., Yim C. (1980). From motivation to Action—Functional interface between the limbic system and the motor system. Progress in Neurobiology 14(2–3): 69–97

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Monchi O., Petrides M., Strafella P., Worsley K., Doyon J. (2006). Functional role of the basal ganglia in the planning and execution of action. Annals of Neurology 59(2): 257–264

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Nisbett R., Wilson T. (1977). Telling more than wecan know: Verbal reports on mental processes. Psychological Review 84, 231–259

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Pascual-Leone A., Grafman J., Hallett M. (1994). Modulation of cortical motor output maps during development of implicit and explicit learning. Science 263, 1287–1289

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Pearson D., Laird J. (2005). Incremental learning of procedural planning knowledge in challenging environment. Computational Intelligence 21(4): 414–439

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Perea M., Gotor A. (1997). Associative and semantic priming effects occur at very short stimulus–onset asynchronies in lexical decision and naming. Cognition 62, 223–240

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Perea M., Rosa E.(2002). The effects of associative and semantic priming in the lexical decision task. Psychological Research 66, 180–194

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Poldrack R., Clark J.,Paré–Blagoev E., Shohamy D., Moyano J.,Myers C., Gluck M. (2001). Interactive memory systems in the human brain. Nature 414, 546–550

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Poldrack R., Sabb F.,Foerde K.,Tom S.,Asarnow R., Bookheimer S., Knowlton B. (2005). The neural correlates of motor skill automaticity. Journal of Neuroscience 25(22): 5356–5364

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Quaney B., Rotella D., Peterson C., Cole K. (2003). Sensorimotor memory for fingertip forces: Evidence for a task-independent motor memory. Journal of Neuroscience 23(5): 1981–1986

    Google Scholar 

  • Quinton A. (1970). Ryle on perception. In: Wood O.P., Pitcher G. (eds) Ryle. GardenCity, Anchor Books

    Google Scholar 

  • Reber P., Squire L.(2005) Encapsulation of implicit and explicit memory in sequence learning. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience 10(2): 248–263

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Reber A. (1989). Implicit Learning and Tacit Knowledge. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General 118(3): 219–235

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Reber A., Lewis C. (1977). Implicit Learning: Ananalysis of the form and strucuture of a body of tacit knowledge. Cognition 5, 333–361

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Rosenbloom P., Laird J., McDermott J., Newell A., Orciuch E. (1985). R1-Soar: An experiment in knowledge-intensive programming in a problem–solving architecture". IEEE Transactions on Pattern Analysis and Machine Intelligence 7, 561–569

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Rumelhart D. (1989). The architecture of the mind: a connectionist approach. In: Posner M. (eds) Foundations of cognitive science. Cambridge, MITPress

    Google Scholar 

  • Ryle G. (1949). The concept of mind. NewYork, Barnesand Noble

    Google Scholar 

  • Saxe R.(2005). Against simulation: Theargumentfromerror. TRENDS in Cognitive Sciences 9, 174–179

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Schacter D.(1989).On the relation between memory and consciousness: Dissociable interactions and conscious experience. In: Roediger H., Craik I. (eds) Varieties of memory and consciousness: Essaysin honorof EndelTulving. Hillsdale, Lawrence Erlbaum Associates

    Google Scholar 

  • Schmand B., Kop W., Kuipers T., Jacquoline B. (1992). Implicit learning in psychotic patients. Schizophrenia Research 7(1): 55–64

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Schröder J.,Tittel A.,Stockert A., Karr M. (1996). Memory deficits in subsyndromes of chronic Schizophrenia. Schizophrenia Research 21(1): 19–26

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Seger C. (1994). Implicit learning. Psychological Bulletin 115(2): 163–196

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Smeets W., Marin O., Gonzalez A. (2000). Evolution of the basal ganglia: New perspectives through a comparative approach. Journal of Anatomy 196(Pt4): 501–17

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Smith C.,Clark R., Manns J., Squire L.(2005). Acquisition of differential delay eyeblink classical conditioning is independent of awareness. Behavioral Neuroscience 119(1): 78–86

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Squire L. (2004). memory systems of the brain: A brief history and current perspective. Neurobiology of Learning and Memory 82(3): 171–177

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Stanley J., Williamson T. (2001). Knowing how. The Journal of Philosophy 98(8): 411–444

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Sun R., Slusarz P., Terry C. (2005). The interaction of the explicit and the implicit in skill learning: Adual-process approach. Psychological Review 112(1): 159–192

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Sternberg R., Horvath J. (1998). Cognitive conceptions of expertise and their relations to giftedness. In R. Friedman, & K. Rogers, (Eds.), Talent in context: Historical and social perspectives on giftedness (pp. 177–191).

  • Stich S., Nichols S. (1997). Cognitive penetrability, rationality, and restricted simulation. Mind & Language 12, 297–326

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Stillings N.,Feinstein M.,Garfield J., Rissland E., Rosenbaum D.,Weisler S., Baker-Ward L. (1987). Cognitive science: An introduction. Cambridge, MITPress

    Google Scholar 

  • Thalen E.(1995). Time-scale dynamics and the development of an embodied cognition. In: Port R., Van Gelder T. (eds) Mind as motion. Cambridge, MITPress

    Google Scholar 

  • Tversky A., Kahneman D. (1973). Availability: A heuristic for judging frequency and probability. Cognitive Psychology 5, 207–232

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Tversky A., Kahneman D. (1974). Judgment under uncertainty: Heuristics and biases. Science 185, 1124–1131

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Unger P.(1967). Experience and factual knowledge. The Journal of Philosophy 64, 152–73

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • van Grop W., Altshuler L.,Theberge D., Mintz J. (1999). Declarative and procedural memory in bipolar disorder. Biological Pyschiatry 46(4): 525–531

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Vicari S., Bellucci S., Carlesimo G. (2001). Procedural learning deficit in children with Williams syndrome. Neuropsychologia 39(7): 665–677

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Waters A., Gobet F., Leyden G. (2002). Visuo-spatial abilities inchess players. British Journal of Psychology 30, 303–311

    Google Scholar 

  • Wells-Jensen S.,Wells-Jensen J., Belknap G. (2005). Changing the public’s attitude toward Braille: Agrass roots approach. Journal of Visual Impairment & Blindness 99, 133–140

    Google Scholar 

  • Wiggins N., Hoffman P. (1968). Three models of clinical judgment. Journal of Abnormal Psychology 73(1): 70–77

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Wilson T., Nisbett R. (1978). The accuracy of verbal reports about the effects of stimuli on evaluations and behavior. Social Psychology 41(2): 118–131

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Yamadori A.,Yoshida T., Mori E. (1996). Neurological basis of skill learning. Cognitive Brain Research 5(1–2): 49–54

    Article  Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Charles Wallis.

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Cite this article

Wallis, C. Consciousness, context, and know-how. Synthese 160, 123–153 (2008). https://doi.org/10.1007/s11229-006-9103-3

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s11229-006-9103-3

Keywords

Navigation