Skip to main content

Advertisement

Log in

Visuomotor integration and executive functioning are uniquely linked to Chinese word reading and writing in kindergarten children

  • Published:
Reading and Writing Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

This cross-sectional study examined the associations of visuomotor integration and executive functioning with Chinese word reading and writing in kindergarten children. A total of 369 Chinese children (mean age = 57.99 months; 55% of them were girls) from Hong Kong, China, completed tasks on visuomotor integration, executive functioning, and Chinese word reading and writing. Children also completed tasks on rapid automatized naming, and their mothers provided child and family background information. Hierarchical regression analyses revealed that, controlling for child age, gender, and rapid automatized naming and maternal education, both visuomotor integration and executive functioning were uniquely linked to Chinese word reading and writing. Findings highlighted the importance of visuomotor integration and executive functioning in understanding the development of Chinese word reading and writing in early years, and the utility of targeting visuomotor integration and executive functioning to help kindergarten children to learn to read and write in Chinese.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Similar content being viewed by others

References

  • Becker, D. R., Miao, A., Duncan, R., & McClelland, M. M. (2014). Behavioral self-regulation and executive function both predict visuomotor skills and early academic achievement. Early Childhood Research Quarterly, 29(4), 411–424.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Beery, K. E., & Beery, N. A. (2004). The Beery-Buktenica developmental test of visual-motor integration (Manual). Bloomington, MN: Pearson Assessments.

    Google Scholar 

  • Beery, K. E., Buktenica, N. A., & Beery, N. A. (1997). The Beery-Buktenica developmental test of visual-motor integration: VMI, with supplemental developmental tests of visual perception and motor coordination: Administration, scoring and teaching manual. New York: Modern Curriculum Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Berlin, L., & Bohlin, G. (2002). Response inhibition, hyperactivity, and conduct problems among preschool children. Journal of Clinical Child and Adolescent Psychology, 31(2), 242–251.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bhattacharya, A. (2010). Children and adolescents from poverty and reading development: A research review. Reading and Writing Quarterly, 26(2), 115–139.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Blair, C. (2016). Developmental science and executive function. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 25(1), 3–7.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Blair, C., & Razza, R. P. (2007). Relating effortful control, executive function, and false belief understanding to emerging math and literacy ability in kindergarten. Child Development, 78(2), 647–663.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Blakemore, S.-J., & Choudhury, S. (2006). Development of the adolescent brain: Implications for executive function and social cognition. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 47(3), 296–312.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bowers, P. G., & Newby-Clark, E. (2002). The role of naming speed within a model of reading acquisition. Reading and Writing, 15(1), 109–126.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Brannigan, G. G., & Decker, S. L. (2003). Bender visual-motor gestalt test (2nd ed.). Itasca, IL: Riverside.

    Google Scholar 

  • Calderon, J., Jambaqué, I., Bonnet, D., & Angeard, N. (2014). Executive functions development in 5-to 7-year-old children with transposition of the great arteries: A longitudinal study. Developmental Neuropsychology, 39(5), 365–384.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Cameron, C. E., Brock, L. L., Hatfield, B. E., Cottone, E. A., Rubinstein, E., LoCasale-Crouch, J., et al. (2015). Visuomotor integration and inhibitory control compensate for each other in school readiness. Developmental Psychology, 51(11), 1529–1543.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Cameron, C. E., Brock, L. L., Murrah, W. M., Bell, L. H., Worzalla, S. L., Grissmer, D., et al. (2012). Fine motor skills and executive function both contribute to kindergarten achievement. Child Development, 83(4), 1229–1244.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Cameron, C. E., Cottone, E. A., Murrah, W. M., & Grissmer, D. W. (2016). How are motor skills linked to children’s school performance and academic achievement? Child Development Perspectives, 10(2), 93–98.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Carlson, A. G., Rowe, E., & Curby, T. W. (2013). Disentangling fine motor skills’ relations to academic achievement: The relative contributions of visual-spatial integration and visual-motor coordination. The Journal of Genetic Psychology, 174(5), 514–533.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Chan, D. W., Ho, C. S. H., Tsang, S. M., Lee, S. H., & Chung, K. K. (2006). Exploring the reading–writing connection in Chinese children with dyslexia in Hong Kong. Reading and Writing, 19(6), 543–561.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Chung, K. K.-H. (2015). Behavioral self-regulation and its contribution to reading among Chinese poor readers. Asia Pacific Journal of Developmental Differences, 2(1), 5–25.

    Google Scholar 

  • Chung, K. K.-H., & McBride-Chang, C. (2011). Executive functioning skills uniquely predict Chinese word reading. Journal of Educational Psychology, 103(4), 909–921.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Chung, K. K.-H., McBride-Chang, C., Wong, S. W., Cheung, H., Penney, T. B., & Ho, C. S.-H. (2008). The role of visual and auditory temporal processing for Chinese children with developmental dyslexia. Annals of Dyslexia, 58(1), 15–35.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Davidson, M. C., Amso, D., Anderson, L. C., & Diamond, A. (2006). Development of cognitive control and executive functions from 4 to 13 years: Evidence from manipulations of memory, inhibition, and task switching. Neuropsychologia, 44(11), 2037–2078.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • De Franchis, V., Usai, M. C., Viterbori, P., & Traverso, L. (2017). Preschool executive functioning and literacy achievement in Grades 1 and 3 of primary school: A longitudinal study. Learning and Individual Differences, 54, 184–195.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Decker, S. L., England, J. A., Carboni, J. A., & Brooks, J. H. (2011). Cognitive and developmental influences in visual-motor integration skills in young children. Psychological Assessment, 23(4), 1010–1016.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Diamond, A. (2013). Executive functions. Annual Review of Psychology, 64, 135–168.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Dilworth-Bart, J. E. (2012). Does executive function mediate SES and home quality associations with academic readiness? Early Childhood Research Quarterly, 27(3), 416–425.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Espy, K. A., Sheffield, T. D., Wiebe, S. A., Clark, C. A., & Moehr, M. J. (2011). Executive control and dimensions of problem behaviors in preschool children. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry and Allied Disciplines, 52(1), 33–46.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Garon, N., Bryson, S. E., & Smith, I. M. (2008). Executive function in preschoolers: A review using an integrative framework. Psychological Bulletin, 134(1), 31–60.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Georgiou, G. K., Parrila, R., Cui, Y., & Papadopoulos, T. C. (2013). Why is rapid automatized naming related to reading? Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 115(1), 218–225.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Georgiou, G. K., Torppa, M., Manolitsis, G., Lyytinen, H., & Parrila, R. (2012). Longitudinal predictors of reading and spelling across languages varying in orthographic consistency. Reading and Writing, 25(2), 321–346.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Grissmer, D., Grimm, K. J., Aiyer, S. M., Murrah, W. M., & Steele, J. S. (2010). Fine motor skills and early comprehension of the world: Two new school readiness indicators. Developmental Psychology, 46(5), 1008–1017.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Guan, C. Q., Perfetti, C. A., & Meng, W. (2015). Writing quality predicts Chinese learning. Reading and Writing, 28(6), 763–795.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hammill, D. D. (2004). What we know about correlates of reading. Exceptional Children, 70(4), 453–469.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ho, C. S.-H., Yau, P. W.-Y., & Au, A. (2003). Development of orthographic knowledge and its relationship with reading and spelling among Chinese kindergarten and primary school children. In C. McBride-Chang & H.-C. Chen (Eds.), Reading development in Chinese children (pp. 51–71). Australia: Greenwood.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hong Kong Census and Statistic Department. (2015). 2015 population by-census. Hong Kong: Hong Kong Special Administrative Region Government.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kao, H. S. R. (2000). The visual-spatial features of Chinese characters and a psychogeometric theory OF Chinese character writing. Hong Kong: Hong Kong University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kao, H. S. (2006). Shufa: Chinese calligraphic handwriting (CCH) for health and behavioural therapy. International Journal of Psychology, 41(4), 282–286.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lan, X., Legare, C. H., Ponitz, C. C., Li, S., & Morrison, F. J. (2011). Investigating the links between the subcomponents of executive function and academic achievement: A cross-cultural analysis of Chinese and American preschoolers. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 108(3), 677–692.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lee, K., Bull, R., & Ho, R. M. (2013). Developmental changes in executive functioning. Child Development, 84(6), 1933–1953.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Liu, I. M., Chuang, C. J., & Wang, S. C. (1975). Frequency count of 40,000 Chinese words. Taipei: Lucky Books.

    Google Scholar 

  • McBride-Chang, C., & Ho, C. S.-H. (2000). Developmental issues in Chinese children’s character acquisition. Journal of Educational Psychology, 92(1), 50–55.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • McBride-Chang, C., & Ho, C. S.-H. (2005). Predictors of beginning reading in Chinese and English: A 2-year longitudinal study of Chinese kindergartners. Scientific Studies of Reading, 9(2), 117–144.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ministry of Education of The People’s Republic of China (2015). Educational statistics in 2015. Retrieved from http://en.moe.gov.cn/Resources/Statistics/edu_stat_2015/2015_en01/.

  • Miyake, A., & Friedman, N. P. (2012). The nature and organization of individual differences in executive functions four general conclusions. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 21(1), 8–14.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Miyake, A., Friedman, N. P., Emerson, M. J., Witzki, A. H., Howerter, A., & Wager, T. D. (2000). The unity and diversity of executive functions and their contributions to complex “frontal lobe” tasks: A latent variable analysis. Cognitive Psychology, 41(1), 49–100.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Monn, A. R., Narayan, A. J., Kalstabakken, A. W., Schubert, E. C., & Masten, A. S. (2017). Executive function and parenting in the context of homelessness. Journal of Family Psychology, 31(1), 61–70.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Nigg, J. T. (2000). On inhibition/disinhibition in developmental psychopathology: Views from cognitive and personality psychology and a working inhibition taxonomy. Psychological Bulletin, 126(2), 220–246.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Norton, E. S., & Wolf, M. (2012). Rapid automatized naming (RAN) and reading fluency: Implications for understanding and treatment of reading disabilities. Annual Review of Psychology, 63, 427–452.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Otto, B. (2008). Literacy development in early childhood: Reflective teaching for birth to age eight. New Jersey: Pearson Education.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ponitz, C. C., McClelland, M. M., Matthews, J. S., & Morrison, F. J. (2009). A structured observation of behavioral self-regulation and its contribution to kindergarten outcomes. Developmental Psychology, 45(3), 605–619.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Rayner, K., Foorman, B. R., Perfetti, C. A., Pesetsky, D., & Seidenberg, M. S. (2001). How psychological science informs the teaching of reading. Psychological Science in the Public Interest, 2(2), 31–74.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Shavers, V. L. (2007). Measurement of socioeconomic status in health disparities research. Journal of the National Medical Association, 99(9), 1013–1020.

    Google Scholar 

  • Shi, B. J., Li, H., Zhang, Y. P., & Shu, H. (2011). The role of logographeme characteristics and orthographic awareness in low-grade children’s writing development. Psychology of Development and Education, 27(3), 297–303.

    Google Scholar 

  • Shu, H., Chen, X., Anderson, R. C., Wu, N., & Xuan, Y. (2003). Properties of school Chinese: Implications for learning to read. Child Development, 74(1), 27–47.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Shu, H., McBride-Chang, C., Wu, S., & Liu, H. (2006). Understanding Chinese developmental dyslexia: Morphological awareness as a core cognitive construct. Journal of Educational Psychology, 98(1), 122–133.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Sigrist, R., Rauter, G., Riener, R., & Wolf, P. (2013). Augmented visual, auditory, haptic, and multimodal feedback in motor learning: A review. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 20(1), 21–53.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Tan, L. H., Spinks, J. A., Eden, G. F., Perfetti, C. A., & Siok, W. T. (2005). Reading depends on writing, in Chinese. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 102(24), 8781–8785.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Tominey, S., & McClelland, M. M. (2011). Red light, purple light: Findings from a randomized trial using circle time games to improve behavioral self-regulation in preschool. Early Education and Development, 22(3), 489–519.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • United Nations (2017). World population prospects 2017. Retrieved from: https://esa.un.org/unpd/wpp/Download/Standard/Population/

  • Wang, Y., McBride-Chang, C., & Chan, S. F. (2014). Correlates of Chinese kindergarteners’ word reading and writing: The unique role of copying skills? Reading and Writing, 27(7), 1281–1302.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Wang, Y., Yin, L., & McBride, C. (2015). Unique predictors of early reading and writing: A one-year longitudinal study of Chinese kindergarteners. Early Childhood Research Quarterly, 32, 51–59.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Welsh, J. A., Nix, R. L., Blair, C., Bierman, K. L., & Nelson, K. E. (2010). The development of cognitive skills and gains in academic school readiness for children from low-income families. Journal of Educational Psychology, 102(1), 43–53.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Whitehurst, G. J., & Lonigan, C. J. (1998). Child development and emergent literacy. Child Development, 69(3), 848–872.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Whitehurst, G. J., & Lonigan, C. (2001). Get ready to read. An early literacy manual: Screening tool, activities and resources.. Pearson: Columbus, OH.

    Google Scholar 

  • Willoughby, M. T., Wirth, R. J., & Blair, C. B. (2012). Executive function in early childhood: Longitudinal measurement invariance and developmental change. Psychological Assessment, 24(2), 418–431.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Yeniad, N., Malda, M., Mesman, J., van IJzendoorn, M. H., Emmen, R. A., & Prevoo, M. J. (2014). Cognitive flexibility children across the transition to school: A longitudinal study. Cognitive Development, 31, 35–47.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Zaitchik, D., Iqbal, Y., & Carey, S. (2014). The effect of executive function on biological reasoning in young children: An individual differences study. Child Development, 85(1), 160–175.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Zelazo, P. D., Müller, U., Frye, D., Marcovitch, S., Argitis, G., Boseovski, J., et al. (2003). The development of executive function in early childhood. Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development, 68(3), 1–151.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Zmigrod, S., & Hommel, B. (2013). Feature integration across multimodal perception and action: A review. Multisensory Research, 26(1–2), 143–157.

    Article  Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgements

This research was supported by a grant by Research Grants Council, University Grants Committee, Hong Kong, to Chun Bun Lam (ECS 28401714). We thanked the undergraduate assistants and faculty collaborators for their help in conducting this study, as well as the participating preschools and families for their time and insight about family relationships and child development.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Kevin Kien Hoa Chung.

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Chung, K.K.H., Lam, C.B. & Cheung, K.C. Visuomotor integration and executive functioning are uniquely linked to Chinese word reading and writing in kindergarten children. Read Writ 31, 155–171 (2018). https://doi.org/10.1007/s11145-017-9779-4

Download citation

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s11145-017-9779-4

Keywords

Navigation