Skip to main content
Log in

Cracking the Chinese character: radical sensitivity in learners of Chinese as a foreign language and its relationship to Chinese word reading

  • Published:
Reading and Writing Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

Radicals are building blocks of Chinese complex characters and exhibit certain positional, phonological and semantic regularities. This study investigated whether adult non-native learners of Mandarin Chinese as a foreign language (CFL) were aware of the positional (orthographic), phonological and semantic information of radicals, and whether such radical sensitivity was predictive to their Chinese word reading abilities. Eighty-four CFL learners were administered a picture-character mapping task in the no cue, phonetic cue and semantic cue conditions, along with two character reading aloud tasks. CFL learners tended to choose the options of correct radicals in correct positions more than the ones containing correct radicals in incorrect positions when no cue was provided. A semantic radical bias was observed in both the no cue and semantic cue conditions: CFL learners chose semantic radicals in correct positions more than phonetic radicals in correct positions. But the pattern was reversed when phonetic cue was provided. In addition, radical sensitivity uniquely predicted CFL learners’ word reading even after controlling for years of learning Mandarin Chinese. Results showed that CFL learners employed orthographic, phonological and semantic information of radicals in encoding novel characters in a manner largely similar to that of native Chinese readers.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Fig. 1
Fig. 2
Fig. 3
Fig. 4

Similar content being viewed by others

Notes

  1. The pronunciation of the characters is expressed by pinyin, a romanization system that is used to spell sound of the simplified characters. The diacritical marks ˉ (high level), ˊ (high rising), ˇ (falling-rising). ˋ (high falling) represent four different tones of Mandarin.

  2. The reliability for the picture-character mapping task was .80 and .70 when it was coded in terms of semantic radicals and phonetic radicals, respectively.

  3. Semantic radical sensitivity and phonetic radical sensitivity referred specifically to participants’ total scores of correct semantic radical-correct position and correct phonetic radical-correct position under no cue condition. No cue condition was counted only because it tapped participant’s initial understanding of radicals without any clue provided.

References

  • Anderson, R. C., Ku, Y. M., Li, W., Chen, X., Wu, X., & Shu, H. (2013). Learning to see the patterns in Chinese characters. Scientific Studies of Reading, 17(1), 41–56.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Anderson, R. C., Li, W., Ku, Y. M., Shu, H., & Wu, N. (2003). Use of partial information in learning to read Chinese characters. Journal of Educational Psychology, 95(1), 52.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bi, Y., Han, Z., & Zhang, Y. (2009). Reading does not depend on writing, even in Chinese. Neuropsychologia, 47, 1193–1199.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Chan, L., & Nunes, T. (1998). Children’s understanding of the formal and functional characteristics of written Chinese. Applied Psycholinguistics, 19(1), 115–131.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Cheung, H., Chan, M. M. N., & Chong, K. K.-Y. (2007). Use of orthographic knowledge in reading by Chinese-English bi-scriptal children. Language Learning, 57, 469–505.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ding, G., Peng, D., & Taft, M. (2004). The nature of the mental representation of radicals in Chinese: A priming study. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 30(2), 530–539.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ding, S., & Saunders, R. A. (2006). Talking up China: An analysis of China’s rising cultural power and global promotion of the Chinese language. East Asia, 23(2), 3–33.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Fan, K. Y., Gao, J. Y., & Ao, X. P. (1984). Pronunciation principles of the Chinese character and alphabetic writing scripts. Chinese Character Reform, 3, 23–27.

    Google Scholar 

  • Feldman, L. B., & Siok, W. W. T. (1999). Semantic radicals contribute to the visual identification of Chinese characters. Journal of Memory and Language, 40(4), 559–576.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Guan, C. Q., Liu, Y., Chan, D. H. L., Ye, F., & Perfetti, C. A. (2011). Writing strengthens orthography and alphabetic-coding strengthens phonology in learning to read Chinese. Journal of Educational Psychology, 103(3), 509.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ho, C. S.-H., Ng, T.-T., & Ng, W.-K. (2003). A radical approach to reading development in Chinese: The role of semantic radicals and phonetic radicals. Journal of Literacy Research, 35(3), 849–878.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hoosain, R. (1991). Psycholinguistic implications for linguistic relativity: A case study of Chinese. New York: Psychology Press.

  • Hoover, W. A., & Gough, P. B. (1990). The simple view of reading. Reading and Writing, 2(2), 127–160.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ke, C. (1998). Effects of language background on the learning of Chinese characters among foreign language students. Foreign Language Annals, 31(1), 91–102.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lau, H. (Ed.). (2010). New practical Chinese reader. Beijing, China: Beijing Language nd Culture University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Law, S. P., & Leung, M. T. (2000). Structural representations of characters in Chinese writing: Evidence from a case of acquired dysgraphia. Psychologia, 43, 67–83.

    Google Scholar 

  • Liu, P. D., Chung, K. K., McBride-Chang, C., & Tong, X. (2010). Holistic versus analytic processing: Evidence for a different approach to processing of Chinese at the word and character levels in Chinese children. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 107(4), 466–478.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Liu, Y., Perfetti, C. A., & Wang, M. (2006). Visual analysis and lexical access of Chinese characters by Chinese as second language readers. Language & Linguistics, 7, 637–657.

  • Liu, Y., Wang, M., & Perfetti, C. A. (2007). Threshold-style processing of Chinese characters for adult second-language learners. Memory & Cognition, 35(3), 471–480.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • McBride-Chang, C. (2004). Children’s literacy development. London: Edward Arnold/Oxford Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Perfetti, C. A., Liu, Y., & Tan, L. H. (2005). The lexical constituency model: Some implications of research on Chinese for general theories of reading. Psychological Review, 112(1), 43–59.

  • Perfetti, C. A., & Tan, L. H. (1998). The time course of graphic, phonological, and semantic activation in Chinese character identification. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 24(1), 101–118.

    Google Scholar 

  • Shen, H. H. (2004). Level of cognitive processing: Effects on character learning among non-native learners of Chinese as a foreign language. Language and Education, 18, 167–182.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Shen, H. H. (2005). An investigation of Chinese-character learning strategies among non-native speakers of Chinese. System, 33(1), 49–68.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Shen, D., & Forster, K. I. (1999). Masked phonological priming in reading Chinese words depends on the task. Language and Cognitive Processes, 14(5–6), 429–459.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Shu, H., Chen, X., Anderson, R. C., Wu, N., & Xuan, Y. (2003). Properties of school Chinese: Implications for learning to read. Child Development, 74(1), 27–47.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Taft, M., & Chung, K. (1999). Using radicals in teaching Chinese characters to second language learners. Psychologia, 42(4), 243–251.

    Google Scholar 

  • Taft, M., & Zhu, X. (1997). Submorphemic processing in reading Chinese. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 23, 761–775.

    Google Scholar 

  • Taft, M., & Zhu, X. (1999). Positional specificity of radicals in Chinese character recognition. Journal of Memory and Language, 40, 498–519.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Taft, M., Zhu, X., & Peng, D. (1999). Positional specificity of radicals in Chinese character recognition. Journal of Memory and Language, 40(4), 498–519.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Tong, X., & McBride, C. (2014). Chinese children’s statistical learning of orthographic regularities: Positional constraints and character structure. Scientific Studies of Reading, (ahead-of-print), 1–18.

  • Tong, X., & McBride-Chang, C. (2010). Developmental models of learning to read Chinese words. Developmental Psychology, 46(6), 1662–1676.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Wang, M., Perfetti, C. A., & Liu, Y. (2003). Alphabetic readers quickly acquire orthographic structure in learning to read Chinese. Scientific Studies of Reading, 97(1), 67–88.

    Google Scholar 

  • Welles, E. B. (2004). Foreign language enrollments in United States institutions of higher education, Fall 2002. ADFL Bulletin, 35(2–3), 413–419.

    Google Scholar 

  • Williams, C., & Bever, T. (2010). Chinese character decoding: A semantic bias? Reading and Writing, 23(5), 589–605.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Wu, N., Zhou, X., & Shu, H. (1999). Sublexical processing in reading Chinese: A development study. Language and Cognitive Processes, 14(5/6), 503–524.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Yin, B., & Rohsenow, J. S. (1994). Modern Chinese characters. Beijing: Sinolingua.

    Google Scholar 

  • Zhou, X., & Marslen-Wilson, W. (1999). Phonology, orthography and semantic activation in reading Chinese. Journal of Memory and Language, 41(4), 579–606.

    Article  Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgments

The authors would like to thank Elsa ZhuNan Chen for her assistance in designing picture-character mapping task. This research was supported, by the Faculty Research Grant from the University of Hong Kong to Dr. Xiuli Tong.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Xiuli Tong.

Appendices

Appendix 1

figure aa

Appendix 2

Single-character word recognition task

1

31

61

便

91

121

151

181

2

32

62

92

122

152

182

3

33

63

93

123

153

183

4

34

64

94

124

154

184

5

35

65

95

125

155

185

6

36

66

96

126

156

186

7

37

67

97

127

157

187

8

38

68

98

128

158

188

9

39

69

99

129

159

189

10

40

70

100

130

160

190

11

41

71

101

131

西

161

191

12

42

72

102

132

162

192

13

43

73

103

133

163

193

14

44

74

104

134

164

194

15

45

75

105

135

165

195

16

46

76

106

136

166

196

17

47

77

107

137

167

197

18

48

78

108

138

168

198

19

49

79

109

139

169

199

20

50

80

110

140

170

200

21

51

81

111

141

171

201

22

52

82

112

142

172

202

23

53

83

113

143

173

203

24

54

84

114

144

174

204

25

55

85

115

145

175

205

26

56

86

116

146

176

206

27

57

87

117

147

177

207

28

58

88

118

148

178

208

29

59

89

119

149

179

209

30

60

90

120

150

180

210

211

241

271

301

331

361

391

212

242

272

302

332

362

392

213

243

273

303

333

363

393

214

244

274

304

334

364

394

宿

215

245

275

305

335

365

395

216

246

276

306

336

366

396

217

247

277

307

337

367

397

218

248

278

308

338

368

  

219

249

279

309

339

369

  

220

250

280

310

340

370

  

221

251

281

311

341

371

  

222

252

282

312

342

372

  

223

253

283

313

343

373

  

224

254

284

314

344

374

  

225

255

285

315

345

375

  

226

256

286

316

346

376

  

227

257

287

317

347

寿

377

  

228

258

288

318

348

378

  

229

259

289

319

349

379

  

230

260

290

320

350

380

  

231

261

291

321

351

381

  

232

262

292

322

352

382

  

233

263

293

323

353

383

  

234

264

294

324

354

384

  

235

265

295

325

355

385

  

236

266

296

326

356

386

  

237

267

297

327

357

387

  

238

268

298

328

358

388

  

239

269

299

329

359

389

  

240

270

300

330

360

390

  

Two-character word recognition

1

31

61

91

121

151

2

32

62

92

122

152

3

33

63

93

123

153

4

34

64

94

124

154

5

35

65

95

125

155

6

36

66

96

126

156

7

37

67

97

127

157

寿

8

38

68

98

128

158

9

39

69

99

129

159

10

40

70

100

130

160

11

41

71

101

131

161

12

42

72

102

132

162

13

43

73

103

133

163

14

44

74

104

134

164

15

45

75

105

135

165

16

46

76

106

136

166

17

47

77

107

137

   

18

48

78

108

138

   

19

49

79

109

139

   

20

50

80

110

140

   

21

51

81

111

141

   

22

52

82

112

142

   

23

53

83

113

143

   

24

54

84

114

144

   

25

55

85

115

145

   

26

56

86

116

146

   

27

57

87

117

147

   

28

58

88

118

148

   

29

59

89

119

149

   

30

60

90

120

150

   

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Tong, X., Yip, J.H.Y. Cracking the Chinese character: radical sensitivity in learners of Chinese as a foreign language and its relationship to Chinese word reading. Read Writ 28, 159–181 (2015). https://doi.org/10.1007/s11145-014-9519-y

Download citation

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s11145-014-9519-y

Keywords

Navigation