Skip to main content
Log in

The component skills underlying reading fluency in adolescent readers: a latent variable analysis

  • Published:
Reading and Writing Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

This study investigated the component skills underlying reading fluency in a heterogeneous sample of 527 eighth grade students. Based on a hypothetical measurement model and successive testing of nested models, structural equation modeling revealed that naming speed, decoding, and language were uniquely associated with reading fluency. These findings suggest that the ability to access and retrieve phonological information from long-term storage is the most important factor in explaining individual differences in reading fluency among adolescent readers. The ability to process meaning and decode novel words was smaller but reliable contributors to reading fluency in adolescent readers.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Institutional subscriptions

Fig. 1
Fig. 2

Similar content being viewed by others

References

  • Alliance for Excellent Education. (2003). The alliance for excellent education’s first annual American high school policy conference: Challenges confronting high schools: Literacy, adequacy, and equity. Washington, DC: Author.

    Google Scholar 

  • Anthony, J. L., Williams, J. M., McDonald, R., Corbitt-Shindler, D., Carlson, C. D., & Francis, D. J. (2006). Phonological processing and emergent literacy in Spanish speaking preschool children. Annals of Dyslexia, 56, 239–270.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Anthony, J. L., Williams, J. M., McDonald, R., & Francis, D. J. (2007). Phonological processing and emergent literacy in younger and older preschool children. Annals of Dyslexia, 57, 113–137.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Baddley, A. (2000). Short-term and long-term working memory. In E. Tulving & F. I. M. Crank (Eds.), The Oxford handbook of memory (pp. 77–92). New York, NY: Oxford University Press Inc.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ben-Dror, I., Pollatsek, A., & Scarpati, S. (1991). Word identification in isolation and in context by college dyslexic students. Brain and Language, 40, 471–490.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bentler, P. M. (1995). EQS structural equations program manual. Encino, CA: Multivariate Software.

    Google Scholar 

  • Berninger, V., Abbott, R. D., Vermeulen, K. (2002). Comparison of faster and slower responders to early intervention in reading: Differentiating feature of their language profiles. Learning Disabilities Quarterly, 25, 59–76.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Biancarosa, G., & Snow, C. E. (2004). Reading next—a vision for action and research in middle and high school literacy: A report to Carnegie Corporation of New York. Washington, DC: Alliance for Excellence in Education.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bowey, J. A. (1985). Contextual facilitation in children’s oral reading in relation to grade and decoding skill. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 40, 23–48.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bowers, P. G. (1993). Text reading and rereading: Determinants of fluency beyond word recognition. Journal of Reading Behavior, 25, 133–153.

    Google Scholar 

  • Brenzitz, Z. (2006). Fluency in reading: Synchronization of processes. Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.

    Google Scholar 

  • Catts, H. W., Adlof, S., Hogan, T. P. (2005). Are specific language impairments and dyslexia distinct disorders? Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 48, 1378–1396.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Catts, H. W., Adlof, S., & Weismer, S. E. (2006). Language deficits in poor comprehenders: A case for the simple view of reading. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 49, 278–293.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Catts, H., Fey, M. E., & Zhang, X. (1999). Language basis of reading and reading disabilities: Evidence from a longitudinal investigation. Scientific Studies of Reading, 3, 331–361.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Catts, H. W., Fey, M. E., & Zhang, X. (2001). Estimating the risk of future reading difficulties in kindergarten children: A research-based model and its clinical implementation. Language, Speech, and Hearing Services in Schools, 31, 38–50.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Catts, H. W., Gillespie, M., Leonard, L., Kail, R. V., & Miller, C. (2002). The role of speed of processing, rapid naming, and phonological awareness in reading achievement. Journal of Learning Disabilities, 35, 32–50.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Chard, D. J., Vaughn, S., & Tyler, B. J. (2002). A synthesis of research on effective interventions for building reading fluency with elementary students with learning disabilities. Journal of Learning Disabilities, 35, 386–406.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Clarke, P., Hulme, C., & Snowling, M. (2005). Individual differences in rapid automatized naming and reading: A response timing analysis. Journal of Research in Reading, 28, 73–86.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Compton, D. L., Olson, R. K., DeFries, J. C., & Pennington, B. F. (2002). Comparing the relationships among two different versions of alphanumeric rapid automatized naming and word level reading skills. Scientific Studies of Reading, 6, 343–368.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Cragg, L., & Nation, K. (2006). Exploring written narrative in children with poor reading comprehension. Educational Psychology, 26, 55–72.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Curtis, M. B. (2002). Adolescent reading: A synthesis of research. Boston: Lesley College, The Center for Special Education.

    Google Scholar 

  • Dollaghan, C., & Campbell, T. (1998). Nonword repetition and child language impairment. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 41, 1136–1146.

    Google Scholar 

  • Dunn, L. M., & Dunn, L. M. (1987). Peabody picture vocabulary test (3rd ed.). Circle Pines, MN: AGS Publishing.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ehri, L. C. (1997). Sight word learning in normal readers, dyslexics. In B. Blachman (Ed.), Foundations of reading acquisition and dyslexia: Implications for early intervention (pp. 163–189). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ehri, L., & McCormick, S. (1998). Phases of word learning: Implications for instruction with delayed and disabled readers. Reading and Writing Quarterly: Overcoming Learning Difficulties, 14, 135–163.

    Google Scholar 

  • Fletcher, J. M., Denton, C. D., & Francis, D. J. (2005). Validity of alternate approaches for the identification of learning disabilities: Operationalizing unexpected underachievement. Journal of Learning Disabilities, 38, 545–552.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Fletcher, J. M., Lyon, G. R., Fuchs, L., & Barnes, L. (2006). Learning disabilities: Integrating science, practice, and policy. New York: Guilford Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Fuchs, L. S., Fuchs, D., Hosp, M. K., & Jenkins, J. R. (2001). Oral reading fluency as an indicator of reading competence: A theoretical, empirical, and historical analysis. Scientific Studies of Reading, 5, 239–256.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Fuchs, L. S., Fuchs, D., & Maxwell, L. (1988). The validity of informal reading comprehension measures. Remedial and Special Education, 9, 20–28.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Gaulin, C., & Campbell, T. (1994). Procedure for assessing verbal working memory in normal school-age children: Some preliminary data. Perceptual and Motor Skills, 79, 55–64.

    Google Scholar 

  • Geva, E., & Zadeh, Z. Y. (2006). Reading efficiency in native English-speaking and English-as-a-second language children: The role of oral proficiency and underlying cognitive-linguistic processes. Scientific Studies of Reading, 10, 31–57.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Grigg, W. S., Daane, M. C., Jin, Y., & Campbell, J. R. (2003). The nation’s report card: Reading 2002 (NCES 2003–521). Washington, DC: National Center for Education Statistics.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hock, M. F., & Deshler, D. D. (2003). Adolescent literacy: Ensuring that no child is left behind. Principal Leadership, 13, 55–61.

    Google Scholar 

  • Jenkins, J. R., Fuchs, L. S., van den Broek, P., Espin, C., & Deno, S. L. (2003). Accuracy and fluency in list and context reading of skills and reading disabled groups: Absolute and relative performance levels. Learning Disabilities Research and Practice, 18, 237–245.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kail, R., & Hall, L. K. (1994). Processing speed, naming speed, and reading. Developmental Psychology, 30, 949–954.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Katzir, T. (2002). Multiple pathways to dysfluent reading: A developmental—componential investigation of the development and breakdown of fluent reading. Unpublished dissertation. Tufts University.

  • Katzir, T., Youngsuk, K., Wolf, M., O’Brien, B., Kennedy, B., Lovett, M., & Morris, R. (2006). Reading fluency: The whole is more than the parts. Annals of Dyslexia, 56, 51–82.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kintsch, W., & van Dijk, T. (1978). Toward a model of text comprehension and production. Psychological Review, 85, 363–394.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kirsch, I. S., Jungeblut, A., Jenkins, L., & Kolstad, A. (1993). Adult literacy in America. A first look at the findings of the National Adult Literacy Survey. (NCES 1993–275). U. S. Department of Education, Office of Educational Research and Improvement. Washington, D.C: U.S. Government Printing Office.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kline, R. B. (2005). Principles and practice of structural equation modeling (2nd ed.). New York: The Guilford Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • LaBerge, D., & Samuels, S. J. (1974). Toward a theory of automatic information processing in reading. Cognitive Psychology, 6, 293–323.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Leach, J. M., Scarborough, H. S., & Rescorla, L. (2003). Late-emerging reading disabilities. Journal of Educational Psychology, 95, 211–224.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Leslie, L., & Caldwell, J. (1995). Qualitative reading inventory-II. New York: Addison-Westly.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lexille (2005). Lexille framework for reading [computer software]. Durham, NC: MetaMetrics, Inc.

  • Manis, F. R., Doi, L. M., & Bhadha, B. (2000). Naming speed, phonological awareness, and orthographic knowledge in second graders. Journal of Learning Disabilities, 33, 325–333.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Mather, N., & Woodcock, R. W. (2001). Examiner’s manual. Woodcock–Johnson III tests of cognitive abilities. Itasca, IL: Riverside Publishing.

    Google Scholar 

  • Misra, M., Katzir, T., & Wolf, M. (2004). Neural systems for rapid automatized naming in skilled readers: Unraveling the RAN-reading relationship. Scientific Studies of Reading, 8, 241–256.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Nation, K., Clarke, P., Marshall, C. M., & Durand, M. (2004). Hidden language impairments in children: Parallels between poor reading comprehension and specific language impairment. Journal of Speech, Hearing, and Language Research, 41, 199–211.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Nicholson, R. I., & Fawcett, A. J. (1999). Developmental dyslexia: The role of the cerebellum. Dyslexia, 5, 155–172.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Perfetti, C. A., Marron, M. A., & Foltz, P. W. (1996). Sources of comprehension failure: Theoretical perspectives and case studies. In C. Cornoldi & J. Oakhill (Eds.), Reading comprehension difficulties: Processes and intervention (pp. 137–165). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum Lawrence Associates.

    Google Scholar 

  • Perie, M., Grigg, W. S., & Donahue, P. L. (2005). The nation’s report card: Reading 2005(NCES 2006–451). U.S. Department of Education, Institute of Education Sciences,National Center for Education Statistics. Washington, DC: U.S. Government Printing Office.

    Google Scholar 

  • Pring, L., & Snowling, M. (1986). Developmental changes in word recognition: An information-processing account. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Experimental Psychology, 38, 395–418.

    Google Scholar 

  • Purvis, K. L., & Tannock, R. (2000). Phonological processing, not inhibition, differentiates ADHD and reading disability. Journal of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, 39, 485–494.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Russel, E. D. (2002). An examination of the nature of reading fluency. Unpublished dissertation. University of Rhode Island.

  • Savage, R., Cornich, K., Manly, T., & Hollis, C. (2006). Cognitive processes in children’s reading and attention: The role of working memory, divided attention, and response inhibition. British Journal of Psychology, 97, 365–385.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Schatschneider, S., Carlson, C. D., Francis, D. J., Foorman, B. R., & Fletcher, J. M. (2002). Relationship of rapid automatized naming and phonological awareness in early reading development: Implications for the double deficit hypothesis. Journal of Learning Disabilities, 35, 245–256.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Schatschneider, C., Fletcher, J. M., Francis, D. J., Carlson, C. D., & Foorman, B. R. (2004). Kindergarten prediction of reading skills: A longitudinal comparative analysis. Journal of Educational Psychology, 96, 265–282.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Semel, E., Wiig, E., & Secord, W. (1995). Clinical evaluation of language fundamentals-3. San Antonio, TX: Psychological Corporation.

    Google Scholar 

  • Shaywitz, S. E., Fletcher, J. M., & Shaywitz, B. A. (1996). A conceptual model and definition of dyslexia: Findings emerging from the Connecticut Longitudinal Study. In J. H. Beichman, N. Cohen, M. M. Konstantareas, & R. Tannock (Eds.). Language, learning, and behavior disorders: Developmental, biological, and clinical perspectives (pp. 199–223). New York, NY: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Stanovich, K. E. (1980). Toward an interactive-compensatory model of individual differences in the development of reading fluency. Reading Research Quarterly, 16, 32–71.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Tallal, P., Miller, S. L., Jenkins, W. M., & Merizenich, M. W. (1997). The role of temporal processing in developmental language-based learning disorders: Research, clinical implications. In B. Blachman (Eds.), Foundations of reading acquisition (pp. 49–66). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Thernstrom, A. M., & Thernstrom, S. (2003). No excuses: Closing the racial gap in learning. New York: Simon and Schuster.

    Google Scholar 

  • Tomblin, J. B., Records, N., Buckwalter, P., Zhang, X., Smith, E., & O’Brien, M. (1997). Prevalence of specific language impairment in kindergarten children. Journal of Speech and Hearing Research, 40, 1245–1260.

    Google Scholar 

  • Tomblin, J. B., Zhang, X., Weis, A., Catts, H., & Weismer, S. (2004). Dimensions of individual differences in communication skills among primary grade children. In M. Rice & S. Warren (Eds.), Developmental language disorders: From phenotypes to etiologies (pp. 53–76). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.

    Google Scholar 

  • Torgesen, J., & Hudson, R. (2006). Reading fluency: Critical issues for struggling readers. In S. J. Samuels & A. Farstrup (Eds.). Reading fluency: The forgotten dimension of reading success. Newark, DE: International Reading Association.

    Google Scholar 

  • Torgesen, J. K., Rashotte, C. A., & Alexander, A. W. (2001). Principles of fluency instruction in reading: Relationships with established empirical outcomes. In M. Wolf (Ed.), Dyslexia, fluency, and the brain (pp. 333–356). Parkton, MD: York Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Torgesen, J., Wagner, R., & Rashotte, C. (1998). Test of word reading efficiency. Austin, TX: Pro-Ed.

    Google Scholar 

  • Torgesen, J., Wagner, R, & Rashotte, C. (2000). Comprehensive test of phonological processing. Austin, TX: Pro-Ed.

    Google Scholar 

  • Torgesen, J. K., Wagner, R. K., Rashotte, C. A., Burgess, S., & Hecht S. (1997). Contributions of phonological awareness and rapid automatic naming ability to the growth of word-reading skills in second- to fifth-grade children. Scientific Studies of Reading, 1, 161–185.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Tunmer, W., & Chapman, J. (1996). Language prediction skill, phonological recoding ability, and beginning reading. In C. Hulme & R. M. Joshi (Eds.), Reading and spelling: Development and disorder. Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.

    Google Scholar 

  • Vaughn, S., Fletcher, J. M., Francis, D., Denton, C., Wanzek, J., Cirino, P., Barth, A. E., Romain, M. Response to intervention with older students with reading difficulties. Learning and Individual Differences, in press.

  • Wagner, R., & Torgesen, J. K. (1987). The nature of phonological processing and its causal role in the acquisition of reading skills. Psychological Bulletin, 101, 192–212.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Wagner, R. K., Torgesen, J. K., Laughon, P., Simmons, K., & Rashotte, C. A. (1993). Development of young readers’ phonological processing abilities. Journal of Educational Psychology, 85, 83–103.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Wagner, R. K., Torgesen, K. K., & Rashotte, C. A. (1999). Comprehensive test of phonological processing. Austin, TX: Pro-Ed.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wagner, R. K., Torgesen, J. K., Rashotte, C. A., Hecht, S. A., Barker, T. A., Burgess, S. R., Donahue, J., & Garon, T. (1997). Changing relations between phonological processing abilities and word-level reading as children develop from beginning to skilled readers: A 5-year longitudinal study. Developmental Psychology, 33, 468–479.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Wallace, G., & Hammil, D. (1994). Comprehensive receptive and expressive vocabulary test. Austin, TX: Pro-Ed.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wechsler, D. (1991). Wechsler intelligence scale for children-III. San Antonio, TX: The Psychological Corporation.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wiederholt, J. L., & Bryant, B. R. (1992). Gray oral reading test-3. Austin, TX: Pro-Ed.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wolf, M., & Bowers, P. G. (1999). The double-deficit hypothesis for the developmental dyslexias. Journal of Educational Psychology, 91, 415–438.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Wolf, M., & Katzir-Cohen, T. (2001). Reading fluency and its intervention. Scientific Studies of Reading, 5, 211–239.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Wolff, P. H. (1993). Impaired temporal resolution in developmental dyslexia. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences, 682, 87–103.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Woodcock, R. (1987). Woodcock reading mastery tests—revised. Circle Pines, MN: American Guidance Service.

    Google Scholar 

  • Woodcock, R. W., McGrew, K. S., & Mather, N. (2001). Woodcock–Johnson III test of cognitive abilities. Itasca, IL: Riverside Publishing.

    Google Scholar 

  • Yuiil, N, & Oakhill, J. V. (1991). Children’s problems in text comprehension. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Amy Elizabeth Barth.

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Cite this article

Barth, A.E., Catts, H.W. & Anthony, J.L. The component skills underlying reading fluency in adolescent readers: a latent variable analysis. Read Writ 22, 567–590 (2009). https://doi.org/10.1007/s11145-008-9125-y

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s11145-008-9125-y

Keywords

Navigation