Skip to main content
Log in

The human extended socio-attentional field and its impairment in borderline personality disorder and in social anxiety disorder

  • Published:
Phenomenology and the Cognitive Sciences Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

Being in the bodily presence of others facilitates important perceptual, social, and informational advantages. For example, it enables direct access to other subjects’ embodied perspectives, motivates intersubjective engagements, and is involved in the construction of shared experiences and joint actions. These advantages are based on and gained through attending to and with others, i.e. they rely on social attention. It is no surprise, therefore, that a growing body of empirical data indicates that social attention is a special attentional state that involves specific behavioral and neural-cognitive properties. Another important feature of the human capacity for social attention, which is highlighted in this article is that in everyday environments social attention considerably extends and enriches the subject’s attentional field. This idea draws on phenomenological considerations and on findings from cognitive science research that suggest that subjects can attend to more than in the center of their attention and that under normal conditions we can gain more features and more situations when attending to the movements, gestures, and facial expressions of others. The influence of others on the structure of our attentional field is evident from situations where others are absent from our daily surroundings. In these circumstances our attentional field narrows and the world transforms into an unfamiliar and sometimes uncanny place. Intriguingly, the same most likely occurs in social pathologies such as BPD (border line personality disorder) and SAD (social anxiety disorder), in which the bodily presence of others does not generate the emotional response we see in healthy humans even though the subject’s basic capacity for social attention is intact.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Similar content being viewed by others

Notes

  1. Some studies suggest that this failure overlaps with the subject’s limited attentional capacity. That is, the richness and broadness of the subject’s attentional field evident from subjects’ primary reports is an illusion; in fact, attentional experiences are very limited and amount only to objects (and situations) that are transferred into the subjects’ working memory, and thus can be reported (e.g. Cohen and Dennett 2011).

  2. I suggest that the term ‘awareness’ that is used in this study should be read instead as ‘attentional awareness,’ as it refers to items that were attended by subjects but did not, as yet gain attentional priority over other items.

  3. Additionally, and importantly, the subject’s growing inclination for social referencing encourages the broadening and sophistication of social-attentional traits, such as rapid detection of other humans’ bodies and faces and the sharing of their perspectives and mental states through direct attention.

  4. See the discussion in part 5.

  5. This is evident, for example, in a situation where I suddenly notice people on the other side of the street run away in fear from an event that is, as yet, obscured from me. In these types of circumstances, I normally do not cross the street to examine the situation at hand, rather; their motor behaviors show me that the environment is unsafe (e.g. Gallagher 2012).

  6. When in the bodily presence of others, for example, we will usually pay less attention to daily objects and occurrences, such as the entrance to the buildings we pass on our way to work, nonetheless, they will still appear to us (to a minimal degree).

  7. And most likely also with post-traumatic stress disorder (PTSD).

  8. See my discussion at the end of this section.

  9. This extends in ASD to the loss of the crucial advantages that social attention normally facilitates (e.g. Matsushima and Kato 2013).

  10. As in the case of schizophrenia (Fuchs 2010).

  11. In fact, in anxious persons for whom the presence of others is disturbing the embodied company of others probably increases these deficits.

  12. In the study by Fletcher-Watson et al. (2008), the fixation of the person in the scene on an inanimate object influenced the participants’ attention, directing their gaze toward this object.

  13. See my discussion of this study in section 2.1 above.

  14. This idea is inspired by pioneering works by Varela (e.g. 1999) and Gallagher (e.g. 1997) that show the way to bring together phenomenological ideas and cognitive science research. The case of the human extended socio attentional field suggests that investigations that are sensitive to the structure of the subjective world and also take into account the ontogenesis of our intersubjective capacities and the neurocognitive underpinning that allow them could be useful in understanding the attentional properties that facilitate interpersonal engagements and their impairments in mental illnesses.

References

  • Bader, O. (2018). Being in a group - the emotional scaffolding of the evolution of collective intentionality. In S. Gissis, E. Lamm, & A. Shavit (Eds.), Landscapes of collectivity in the life science (pp. 155–168). Cambridge: MIT press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bader, O., & Peri-Bader, A. (2016). The bodily other and everyday experience of the lived urban world. Journal of Aesthetics and Phenomenology, 2, 93–109.

    Google Scholar 

  • Becchio, C., Manera, V., Sartori, L., Cavallo, A., & Castiello, U. (2012). Grasping intentions: from thought experiments to empirical evidence. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, 6, 117.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bertsch, K., Volman, I., Roelofs, K., Herpertz, S., & Müller, L. E. (2017a). Social emotional leaning and behavioral tendencies in high and low socially anxious men and women. Psychoneuroendocrinology, 83, 49.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bertsch, K., Krauch, M., Stopfer, K., Haeussler, K., Herpertz, S. C., & Gamer, M. (2017b). Interpersonal threat sensitivity in borderline personality disorder: an eye-tracking study. Journal of Personality Disorders, 31, 647–670.

    Google Scholar 

  • Birmingham, E., & Kingstone, A. (2009). Human social attention: a new look at past, present and future investigations. The Year in Cognitive Neuroscience: Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences, 1156, 118–140.

    Google Scholar 

  • Block, N. (2014). Rich conscious perception outside focal attention. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 18(9), 445–447.

    Google Scholar 

  • Boothby, E. J., Clark, M. S., & Bargh, J. A. (2014). Shared experiences are amplified. Psychological Science, 25, 2209–2216.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bronfman, Z., Brezis, N., Hilla Jacobson, H., & Usher, M. (2014). We see more than we can report: cost free color phenomenality outside focal attention. Psychological Science, 25(7), 1394–1403.

    Google Scholar 

  • Brosch, T. (2014). Neurocognitive mechanisms of attentional prioritization in social interactions. In C. Von Scheve & M. Salmela (Eds.), Collective emotions (pp. 78–93). Oxford: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Chabris, C. F., Weinberger, A., Fontaine, M., & Simons, D. J. (2011). You do not talk about fight club if you do not notice fight club: Inattentional blindness for a simulated real-world assault. Perception, 2, 150–153.

    Google Scholar 

  • Chang, Y. S., Owen, J. P., Desai, S. S., Hill, S. S., Arnett, A. B., Harris, J., & Mukherjee, P. (2014). Autism and sensory processing disorders: shared white matter disruption in sensory pathways but divergent connectivity in social-emotional pathways. PLoS One. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0103038.

    Google Scholar 

  • Chen, Z. (2012). Object-based attention: a tutorial review. Attention, Perception, & Psychophysics, 74, 784–802.

    Google Scholar 

  • Cohen, M. A., & Dennett, D. C. (2011). Consciousness cannot be separated from function. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 15, 358–364.

    Google Scholar 

  • Crouzet, S. M., Kirchner, H., & Thorpe, S. J. (2010). Fast saccades toward faces: Face detection in just100 ms. Journal of Vision, 10(4), 1–17.

    Google Scholar 

  • Eimer, M., & Grubert, A. (2015). A dissociation between selective attention and conscious awareness in the representation of temporal order information. Consciousness and Cognition, 35, 274–281.

    Google Scholar 

  • Farroni, T., Csibra, G., Simion, F., & Johnson, M. H. (2002). Eye contact delectation in humans from birth. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 99, 9602–9605.

    Google Scholar 

  • Fletcher-Watson, S., Findlay, J. M., Leekam, S. R., & Benson, V. (2008). Rapid detection of person information in a naturalistic scene. Perception, 37(4), 571–583.

    Google Scholar 

  • Fonagy, P., Target, M., & Gergely, G. (2000). Attachment and borderline personality disorder: a theory and some evidence. Psychiatric Clinics of North America, 23, 103–122.

    Google Scholar 

  • Fuchs, T. (2010). Phenomenology and psychopathology. In S. Gallagher & D. Schmicking (Eds.), Handbook of phenomenology and the cognitive sciences (pp. 547–573). Dordrecht: Springer.

    Google Scholar 

  • Fuchs, T. (2015). Pathologies of Intersubjectivity in autism and schizophrenia. Journal of Consciousness Studies, 22(1–2), 191–214.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gallagher, S. (1997). Mutual enlightenment: recent phenomenology in cognitive science. Journal of Consciousness Studies, 4(3), 195–214.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gallagher, S. (2012). Interactive coordination in joint attention. In A. Seemann (Ed.), Joint attention: New developments in philosophy, psychology, and neuroscience (pp. 293–306). Cambridge: MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gallagher, S., & Allen, M. (2016). Active inference, enactivism and the hermeneutics of social cognition. Syntheses, 195, 2627–2648. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11229-016-1269-8.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Gallagher, S., & Hutto, D. (2008). Understanding others through primary interaction and narrative practice. In J. Zlatev et al. (Eds.), The shared mind: Perspectives on Intersubjectivity (pp. 17–38). Amsterdam: John Benjamins.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gallagher, S., & Zahavi, D. (2008). The phenomenological mind, an introduction to philosophy of mind and cognitive science. New York and London: Routledge.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gallagher, S., & Zahavi, D. (2014). Primal Impression and Enactive Perception. In V. Arstila & D. Lloyd (Eds.), Subjective time: the philosophy, psychology, and neuroscience of temporality (pp. 83–99). Cambridge: MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gallese, V. (2007). Embodied simulation: from mirror neuron systems to interpersonal relations. Novartis Foundation Symposium, 278, 3–12 discussion 12-9.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gallese, V., & Sinigaglia, C. (2010). The bodily self as power for action. Neuropsychologia, 48, 746–755.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gibson, J. J. (1979). The ecological approach to visual perception. Boston: Houghton-Mifflin.

  • Gotlib, I. H., Krasnoperova, E., Yue, D. N., & Joormann, J. (2004). Attentional biases for negative interpersonal stimuli in clinical depression. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 113, 127–135.

    Google Scholar 

  • Guangming, R., Xu, C., Xiaojun, C., & Qi, Z. (2016). Prediction and unconscious attention operate synergistically to facilitate stimulus processing: an fMRI study. Consciousness and Cognition, 44, 41–50.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gurwitsch, A. (1966). Phenomenology and psychology. Evanston: Northwestern University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gurwitsch, A. (1979). Human encounters in the social world. Pittsburg: Duquesne University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hayes, J. P., Vanelzakker, M. B., & Shin, L. M. (2012). Emotion and cognition interactions in PTSD: a review of neurocognitive and neuroimaging studies. Frontiers in Integrative Neuroscience, 6, 89.

    Google Scholar 

  • Heyes, C. (2010). Where do mirror neurons come from? Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews, 34(4), 575–583.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hobson, J., & Hobson, R. P. (2007). Identification: the missing link between joint attention and imitation. Development and Psychopathology, 19, 411–431.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hobson, R. P., Lee, A., & Brown, R. (1999). Autism and congenital blindness. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 12, 45–66.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hoehl, S., & Striano, T. (2010). The development of emotional face and eye gaze processing. Developmental Science, 13(6), 813–825.

    Google Scholar 

  • Husserl, E. (1962) [1913]. Ideas general introduction to pure phenomenology. W.R Gibson-Boyce (Trans). New York: Collier Books.

  • Husserl, E. (1970) [1936]. The crisis of European sciences and transcendental phenomenology: an introduction to phenomenological philosophy. D. Carr (introduction and translation), Evanston: Northwestern University.

  • Iverach, L., & Rapee, R. M. (2014). Social anxiety disorder and stuttering: current status and future directions. Journal of Fluency Disorders, 40, 69–82.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kang-Seob, O., Woo Hyung, L., Sunyoung, K., Dong-Won, S., Young-Chul, S., & Se-Won, L. (2018). Impaired facial expression recognition in patients with social anxiety disorder: a case-control study. Cognitive Neuropsychiatry, 23(4), 218–228.

    Google Scholar 

  • Klinnert, M. D., Campos, J. J., Sorce, J. F., Emde, R. N., & Svejda, M. (1983). Emotions as behavior regulators: social referencing in infants. In R. Plutchik & H. Kellerman (Eds.), Emotion theory, research, and experience (Emotions in early development) (Vol. 2, pp. 57–68). New York: Academic Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Knoblich, G., & Sebanz, N. (2008). Evolving intentions for social interaction: from entrainment to joint action. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society, London. B: Biological Sciences, 363(1499), 2021–2031.

    Google Scholar 

  • Knoblich, G., Butterfill, S., & Sebanz, N. (2011). Psychological research on joint action: theory and data. In B. Ross (Ed.), The psychology of learning and motivation (Vol. 54, pp. 59–101). Burlington: Academic Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Krueger, J. (2012). Seeing mind in action. Phenomenology and the Cognitive Sciences, 11(2), 149–173.

    Google Scholar 

  • Krueger, J. (2013). Ontogenesis of the socially extended mind. Cognitive Systems Research, 25-26, 40–46.

    Google Scholar 

  • Krueger, J., & Colombetti, G. (2018). Affective affordances and psychopathology. Discipline Filosofiche, Special Issue: Philosophical Perspectives on Affectivity and Psychopathology, 18(2), 221–247.

  • Lamy, D., Leber, A.B., & Egeth, H.E. (2012). Selective attention. In A.F. Healy & R.W. Proctor (Eds.) Experimental psychology. Volume 4 of the comprehensive handbook of psychology, Fourth Edition (pp. 265–294). Editor-in-Chief: I. B. Weiner. New York.

  • Langton, S. R. H., Watt, R. J., & Bruce, V. (2000). Do the eyes have it? Cues to the direction of social attention. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 4(2000), 50–59.

    Google Scholar 

  • Mancke, F., Herpertz, S. C., & Bertsch, K. (2015). Aggression in borderline personality disorder– a multidimensional model. Personality Disorders, Theory, Research, and Treatment, 6, 278–291.

    Google Scholar 

  • Matsushima, K., & Kato, T. (2013). Social interaction and atypical sensory processing in children with autism spectrum disorders. Hong Kong Journal of Occupational Therapy, 23(2), 89–96.

    Google Scholar 

  • Morand, S. M., Harvey, M., & Marie-Hélène Grosbras, M. H. (2012). Parieto-occipital cortex shows early target selection to faces in a reflexive orienting task. Cerebral Cortex, 24, 898–907.

    Google Scholar 

  • Mundy, P., Sigman, M., & Kasari, C. (2009). A parallel and distributed-processing model of joint attention, social cognition and autism. Autism Research, 2, 2–21.

    Google Scholar 

  • Murray, L., de Rosnay, M., Pearson, J., Bergeron, C., Schofield, E., Royal-Lawson, M., & Cooper, P. J. (2008). Intergenerational transmission of social anxiety: the role of social referencing processes in infancy. Child Development, 79, 1049–1064.

    Google Scholar 

  • Niedtfeld, I., Defiebre, N., Regenbogen, C., Mier, D., Fenske, S., Kirsch, P., et al. (2017). Facing the problem: impaired emotion recognition during multimodal social information processing in borderline personality disorder. Journal of Personality Disorders, 30, 248–264.

    Google Scholar 

  • Peltola, M. J., Leppänen, J. M., Maki, S., & Hietanen, J. K. (2009). Emergence of enhanced attention to fearful faces between 5 and 7 months of age. Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience, 4, 134–142.

    Google Scholar 

  • Pitcher, D., Goldhaber, T., Duchaine, B., Walsh, V., & Kanwisher, N. (2012). Two critical and functionally distinct stages of face and body perception. The Journal of Neuroscience, 32(45), 15877–15885.

    Google Scholar 

  • Posner, M. I. (1980). Orienting of attention. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 32, 3–25.

    Google Scholar 

  • Reddy, V. (2010). Engaging minds in the first year: the developing awareness of attention and intention. In G. Bremner (Ed.), Handbook of infant development (2nd ed., pp. 365–393). Oxford: Wiley-Blackwell.

    Google Scholar 

  • Reddy, V. (2012). A gaze at grips with me. In A. Seemann (Ed.), Joint attention: New developments in philosophy, psychology, and neuroscience (pp. 137–157). Cambridge: MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rochat, P. (2010). The innate sense of the body develops to become a public affair by 2-3 years. Neuropsychologia, 48(3), 738–745.

    Google Scholar 

  • Salice, A., & Henriksen, M. (2015). The disrupted we: schizophrenia and collective intentionality. Journal of Consciousness Studies, 22(7–8), 145–171.

    Google Scholar 

  • Sartori, L., Cavallo, A., Bucchioni, G., & Castielloet, U. (2011). Corticospinal excitability is specifically modulated by the social dimension of observed actions. Experimental Brain Research, 211, 557–568.

    Google Scholar 

  • Sartre, J.P. (2003). [1943]. Being and nothingness. Hazel E. Barnes (trans.). London: Routledge.

  • Schilbach, L., Timmermans, B., Reddy, V., Costall, A., Bente, G., Schlicht, T., & Vogeley, K. (2013). Toward a second-person neuroscience. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 36(4), 393–414.

    Google Scholar 

  • Schutz, A. (1967) [1932]. The phenomenology of the social world. G. Walsh (trans.), Evanston, IL: Northwestern University press.

  • Shteynberg, G., Hirsh, J. B., Apfelbaum, E. P., Larsen, J. T., Galinsky, A. D., & Roese, N. J. (2014). Feeling more together: group attention intensifies emotion. Emotion. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0037697 Advance online publication.

    Google Scholar 

  • Silvia, P. J., Allan, W. D., Beauchamp, D. L., Maschauer, E. L., & Workman, J. O. (2006). Biased recognition of happy facial expressions in social anxiety. Journal of Social and Clinical Psychology, 25, 585–602.

    Google Scholar 

  • Simons, D. J., & Chabris, C. F. (1999). Gorillas in our midst: sustained inattentional blindness for dynamic events. Perception, 28, 1059–1074.

    Google Scholar 

  • Sinigaglia, C. (2008). Enactive understanding and motor intentionality. In F. Morganti et al. (Eds.), Enacting intersubjectivity: A cognitive and social perspective to study of interactions (pp. 17–32). Amsterdam: IOS Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Stern, D. N. (1985). The interpersonal world of the infant. New York: Basic Books.

    Google Scholar 

  • Striano, T., Kopp, F., Grossmann, T., & Reid, V. M. (2006). Eye contact influences neural processing of emotional expressions in 4-month-old infants. Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience, 1(2), 87–94.

    Google Scholar 

  • Taylor, T. F. (2015). The influence of shame on posttrauma disorders: have we failed to see the obvious? European Journal of Psychotraumatology, 6. https://doi.org/10.3402/ejpt.v6.28847.

    Google Scholar 

  • Thoma, S., & Fuchs, T. (2018). Inhabiting the shared world: phenomenological considerations on sensus communis, social space and schizophrenia. In I. Hipólito, J. Gonçalves, & J. G. Pereira (Eds.), Schizophrenia and common sense explaining the relation between madness and social values (pp. 19–36). Dordrecht: Springer.

    Google Scholar 

  • Tomasello, M. (2009). Why we cooperate. Cambridge: MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Tomasello, M. (2014). A natural history of human thinking. Cambridge: Harvard University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Tomasello, M., & Haberl, K. (2003). Understanding attention: 12- and 18-month-olds know what is new for other persons. Developmental Psychology, 39(5), 906–912.

    Google Scholar 

  • Varela, F. J. (1999). Present-time consciousness. Journal of Consciousness Studies, 6(2–3), 111–140.

    Google Scholar 

  • Walden, T. A., & Baxter, A. (1989). The effect of context and age on social referencing. Child Development, 60(6), 1511–1518.

    Google Scholar 

  • Yu, C., & Smith, L. B. (2013). Joint attention without gaze following: human infants and their parents coordinate visual attention to objects through eye-hand coordination. PLoS One, 8(11), e79659.

    Google Scholar 

  • Zahavi, D. (2015). You, me and we: the sharing of emotional experiences. Journal of Consciousness Studies, 22(1–2), 84–101.

    Google Scholar 

  • Zahavi, D. (2016). Second-person engagement, self-alienation, and group-identification. Topoi, 38, 251–260. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11245-016-9444-6.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Zarbatany, L., & Lamb, M. E. (1985). Social referencing as a function of information source: Mothers versus strangers. Infant Behavior and Development, 8, 25–33.

    Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgments

I am very grateful to Thomas Fuchs, for his help, comments and useful suggestions. I am also deeply thankful to the Minerva foundation for their generous support.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Oren Bader.

Ethics declarations

Conflict of interest

The authors declare that they have no conflict of interest.

Additional information

Publisher’s note

Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Bader, O. The human extended socio-attentional field and its impairment in borderline personality disorder and in social anxiety disorder. Phenom Cogn Sci 19, 169–189 (2020). https://doi.org/10.1007/s11097-019-09621-w

Download citation

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s11097-019-09621-w

Keywords

Navigation