Skip to main content
Log in

An Examination of the Communication Styles Associated with Psychopathy and Their Influence on Observer Impressions

  • Original Paper
  • Published:
Journal of Nonverbal Behavior Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

Psychopathic individuals are characterized as “intra-species predators”—callous, impulsive, aggressive, and proficient at interpersonal manipulation. For example, despite their high risk for re-offending, psychopathic offenders often receive early release on parole. While reputed to be social chameleons, research suggests that even naive observers can accurately infer high levels of psychopathic traits in others with very brief exposures to behavior, but accuracy degrades with extended observation. We utilized a lens model approach to examine the communication styles (emotional facial expressions, body language, and verbal content) of offenders varying in levels of psychopathic traits using “thin slice” video clips of psychological assessment interviews and to reveal which cues observers use to inform their evaluations of psychopathy. Psychopathic traits were associated with more (a) Duchenne smiles, (b) negative (angry) emotional language, and (c) hand gestures (illustrators). Further, psychopathy was associated with a marked behavioral incongruence; when individuals scoring high in psychopathic traits engaged in Duchenne smiles they were also more likely to use angry language. Naïve observers relied on each of these valid behavioral signals to quickly and accurately detect psychopathic traits. These findings provide insight into psychopathic communication styles, opportunities for improving the detection of psychopathic personality traits, and may provide an avenue for understanding successful psychopathic manipulation.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Fig. 1
Fig. 2
Fig. 3
Fig. 4

Similar content being viewed by others

Notes

  1. Participants in Fowler et al.’s (2009) study also watched six 2-s clips in a combined audio–video modality. Due to the small number, and extremely brief nature of these videos, they were not included in the present study.

  2. Achievement (accuracy) correlations reported by Fowler et al. (2009) are slightly different than those reported here, because we use a smaller sub-set (N = 88) of their original sample (N = 96).

References

  • Babiak, P., & Hare, R. D. (2006). Snakes in suits: When psychopaths go to work. New York: Regan Books/Harper Collins Publishers.

    Google Scholar 

  • Biesanz, J. C., West, S. G., & Millevoi, A. (2007). What do you learn about someone over time? The relationship between length of acquaintance and consensus and self-other agreement in judgments of personality. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 92, 119–135.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Black, P. J., Woodworth, M., & Porter, S. (2014). The big bad wolf? The relation between the Dark Triad and the interpersonal assessment of vulnerability. Personality and Individual Differences, 67, 52–56.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Book, A., Methot, T., Gauthier, N., Hosker-Field, A., Forth, A., Quinsey, V., et al. (2015). The Mask of Sanity revisited: Psychopathic traits and affective mimicry. Evolutionary Psychological Science, 1, 91–102.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Brunswik, E. (1956). Perception and the representative design of psychological experiments (2nd ed.). Berkeley: University of California Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Burgoon, J. K. (1993). Interpersonal expectations, expectancy violations, and emotional communication. Journal of Language and Social Psychology, 12, 30–48.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Carney, D. R., Colvin, C. R., & Hall, J. A. (2007). A thin slice perspective on the accuracy of first impressions. Journal of Research in Personality, 41, 1054–1072.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Carney, D. R., Hall, J. A., & LeBeau, L. S. (2005). Beliefs about the nonverbal expression of social power. Journal of Nonverbal Behavior, 29, 105–123.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Cleckley, H. (1946). Psychopath: A problem for society. Federal Probation, 10, 22–25.

    Google Scholar 

  • DePaulo, B. M., Lindsay, J. J., Malone, B. E., Muhlenbruck, L., Charlton, K., & Cooper, H. (2003). Cues to deception. Psychological Bulletin, 129, 74–118.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Dunbar, N. E., & Burgoon, J. K. (2005). Perceptions of power and interactional dominance in interpersonal relationships. Journal of Social and Personal Relationships, 22, 207–233.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ekman, P. (1992). Telling lies. New York: Norton. (Original work published 1985).

    Google Scholar 

  • Ekman, P., Davidson, R. J., & Friesen, W. V. (1990). The Duchenne smile: Emotional expression and brain physiology: II. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 58, 342–353. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.58.2.342.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Ekman, P., Friesen, W. V., & Hagar, J. C. (2002). Facial action coding system. Salt Lake City, UT: Network Information Research. (Original work published 1976).

    Google Scholar 

  • Fowler, K. A., Lilienfeld, S. O., & Patrick, C. J. (2009). Detecting psychopathy from thin slices of behavior. Psychological Assessment, 21, 68–78. doi:10.1037/a0014938.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Fowles, D. C., & Dindo, L. (2006). A dual-deficit model of psychopathy. In C. J. Patrick (Ed.), Handbook of psychopathy (pp. 14–34). New York: Guilford Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Friesen, W. V., Ekman, P., & Wallbott, H. (1979). Measuring hand movements. Journal of Nonverbal Behavior, 4, 97–112.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Gillespie, S. M., Rotshtein, P., Wells, L. J., Beech, A. R., & Mitchell, I. J. (2015). Psychopathic traits are associated with reduced attention to the eyes of emotional faces among adult male non-offenders. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience. doi:10.3389/fnhum.2015.00552.

    PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Gillstrom, B. J., & Hare, R. D. (1988). Language-related hand gestures in psychopaths. Journal of Personality Disorders, 2, 21–27. doi:10.1521/pedi.1988.2.1.21.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Gunnery, S. D., Hall, J. A., & Ruben, M. A. (2013). The deliberate Duchenne smile: Individual differences in expressive control. Journal of Nonverbal Behavior, 37, 29–41.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Gunnery, S. D., & Ruben, M. A. (2016). Perceptions of Duchenne and non-Duchenne smiles: A meta-analysis. Cognition and Emotion, 30, 501–515.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Häkkänen-Nyholm, H., & Hare, R. D. (2009). Psychopathy, homicide, and the courts working the system. Criminal Justice and Behavior, 36, 761–777.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hall, J. A., Coats, E. J., & LeBeau, L. S. (2005). Nonverbal behavior and the vertical dimension of social relations: A meta-analysis. Psychological Bulletin, 131, 898–924.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Hancock, J., Woodworth, M. T., & Porter, S. (2013). Hungry like the wolf: A word pattern analysis of the language of psychopaths. Legal and Criminological Psychology, 18, 102–114.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hare, R. D. (2003). Manual for the revised psychopathy checklist (2nd ed.). Toronto, ON: Multi-Health Systems.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hare, R. D., Clark, D., Grann, M., & Thornton, D. (2000). Psychopathy and the predictive validity of the PCL-R: An international perspective. Behavioral Sciences & the Law, 18, 623–645.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hare, R. D., Hart, S. D., & Harpur, T. J. (1991). Psychopathy and the DSM-IV criteria for antisocial personality disorder. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 100, 391–398.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Hayes, A. F., & Krippendorff, K. (2007). Answering the call for a standard reliability measure for coding data. Communication Methods and Measures, 1, 77–89.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hayes, A. F., & Matthes, J. (2009). Computational procedures for probing interactions in OLS and logistic regression: SPSS and SAS implementations. Behavior Research Methods, 41, 924–936.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Johnston, L., Miles, L., & Macrae, C. N. (2010). Why are you smiling at me? Social functions of enjoyment and non-enjoyment smiles. British Journal of Social Psychology, 49, 107–127.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Kasl, S. V., & Mahl, G. F. (1965). Relationship of disturbances and hesitations in spontaneous speech to anxiety. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 1, 425–433.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Kaufmann, G., Drevland, G. C., Wessel, E., Overskeid, G., & Magnussen, S. (2003). The importance of being earnest: Displayed emotions and witness credibility. Applied Cognitive Psychology, 17, 21–34.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Klaver, J. R., Lee, Z., & Hart, S. D. (2007). Psychopathy and nonverbal indicators of deception in offenders. Law and Human Behavior, 31, 337–351.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Kraha, A., Turner, H., Nimon, K., Zientek, L. R., & Henson, R. K. (2012). Tools to support interpreting multiple regression in the face of multicollinearity. Frontiers in Psychology, 3(44), 1–16.

    Google Scholar 

  • Krumhuber, E. G., & Manstead, A. S. (2009). Can Duchenne smiles be feigned? New evidence on felt and false smiles. Emotion, 9, 807.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Lilienfeld, S. O., & Andrews, B. P. (1996). Development and preliminary validation of a self-report measure of psychopathic personality traits in noncriminal population. Journal of Personality Assessment, 66, 488–524.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Lilienfeld, S. O., Patrick, C. J., Benning, S. D., Berg, J., Sellbom, M., & Edens, J. F. (2012). The role of fearless dominance in psychopathy: Confusions, controversies, and clarifications. Personality Disorders: Theory, Research, and Treatment, 3, 327–340.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lilienfeld, S. O., Watts, A. L., & Smith, S. F. (2015). Successful psychopathy A scientific status report. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 24, 298–303.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lynam, D. R., & Miller, J. D. (2012). Fearless dominance and psychopathy: A response to Lilienfeld et al. Personality Disorders: Theory, Treatment, and Research, 3, 241–353.

    Google Scholar 

  • Oberlander, J., & Gill, A. J. (2006). Language with character: A stratified corpus comparison of individual differences in e-mail communication. Discourse Processes, 42, 239–270.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Patrick, C. J., & Bernat, E. M. (2009). Neurobiology of psychopathy: A two-process theory. In G. G. Berntson & J. T. Cacioppo (Eds.), Handbook of neuroscience for the behavioral sciences (pp. 1110–1131). New York: Wiley.

    Google Scholar 

  • Patrick, C. J., Fowles, D. C., & Krueger, R. F. (2009). Triarchic conceptualization of psychopathy: Developmental origins of disinhibition, boldness, and meanness. Development and Psychopathology, 21, 913–938.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Pennebaker, J. W., Booth, R. E., & Francis, M. E. (2007). Linguistic inquiry and word count: LIWC2007—Operator’s Manual. Austin, TX: LIWC.net.

    Google Scholar 

  • Pennebaker, J. W., & Graybeal, A. (2001). Patterns of natural language use: Disclosure, personality, and social integration. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 10, 90–93.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Pennebaker, J. W., Mehl, M. R., & Niederhoffer, K. G. (2003). Psychological aspects of natural language use: Our words, our selves. Annual Review of Psychology, 54, 547–577.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Porter, S., Bhanwer, A., Woodworth, M., & Black, P. J. (2014). Soldiers of misfortune: An examination of the Dark Triad and the experience of schadenfreude. Personality and Individual Differences, 67, 64–68.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Porter, S., Doucette, N. L., Woodworth, M., Earle, J., & MacNeil, B. (2008). Halfe the world knowes not how the other halfe lies: Investigation of verbal and non-verbal signs of deception exhibited by criminal offenders and non-offenders. Legal and Criminological Psychology, 13, 27–38. doi:10.1348/135532507X186653.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Porter, S., & ten Brinke, L. (2008). Reading between the lies: Identifying concealed and falsified emotions in universal facial expressions. Psychological Science, 19, 508–514. doi:10.1111/j.1467-9280.2008.02116.x.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Porter, S., ten Brinke, L., Baker, A., & Wallace, B. (2011). Would I lie to you? “Leakage” in deceptive facial expressions relates to psychopathy and emotional intelligence. Personality and Individual Differences, 51, 133–137. doi:10.1016/j.paid.2011.03.031.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Porter, S., ten Brinke, L., & Wallace, B. (2012). Secrets and lies: Involuntary leakage in deceptive facial expressions as a function of emotional intensity. Journal of Nonverbal Behavior, 36, 23–37.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Porter, S., ten Brinke, L., & Wilson, K. (2009). Crime profiles and conditional release performance of psychopathic and non-psychopathic sexual offenders. Legal and Criminological Psychology, 14, 109–118. doi:10.1348/135532508X284310.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Reynolds, D. J., & Gifford, R. (2001). The sounds and sights of intelligence: A lens model channel analysis. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 27, 187–200.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Rimé, B., Bouvy, H., Leborgne, B., & Rouillon, F. (1978). Psychopathy and nonverbal behavior in an interpersonal situation. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 87, 636–643. doi:10.1037/0021-843X.87.6.636.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Schmid Mast, M. S., & Hall, J. A. (2004). Who is the boss and who is not? Accuracy of judging status. Journal of Nonverbal Behavior, 28, 145–165.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Soussignan, R. (2002). Duchenne smile, emotional experience, and autonomic reactivity: A test of the facial feedback hypothesis. Emotion, 2, 52–74.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Trope, Y., & Liberman, N. (2003). Temporal construal. Psychological Review, 110, 403–421.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Winston, J. S., Strange, B. A., O’Doherty, J., & Dolan, R. J. (2002). Automatic and intentional brain responses during evaluation of trustworthiness of faces. Nature Neuroscience, 5, 277–283.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Zuckerman, M., DePaulo, B. M., & Rosenthal, R. (1981). Verbal and nonverbal communication of deception. In L. Berkowitz (Ed.), Advances in experimental social psychology (Vol. 14, pp. 1–59). New York: Academic Press.

    Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Leanne ten Brinke.

Appendix

Appendix

Definitions of Factor 1, Factor 2, and Overall Psychopathy, provided to naïve raters:

  • Factor 1—People high in Factor 1 psychopathy tend to have an inflated sense of self-importance, to be “smooth talkers” and to lie and manipulate others without feeling guilty. They lack empathy for other people, and rarely accept responsibility for the things they do wrong.

  • Factor 2—People high in Factor 2 psychopathy have a long history of behavior problems, beginning with juvenile delinquency. They are impulsive, frequently seek stimulation because of boredom, lack realistic long-term goals of their own, and often find ways to live off the support of parents, friends, and sexual partners.

  • Overall—Psychopaths tend to be charming and engaging on the surface, but often manipulate, lie to, and exploit others to get what they want, without guilt or empathy. They tend to be irresponsible and to lack impulse control.

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

ten Brinke, L., Porter, S., Korva, N. et al. An Examination of the Communication Styles Associated with Psychopathy and Their Influence on Observer Impressions. J Nonverbal Behav 41, 269–287 (2017). https://doi.org/10.1007/s10919-017-0252-5

Download citation

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s10919-017-0252-5

Keywords

Navigation