Skip to main content
Log in

The rationality of literal Tide Pod consumption

  • Published:
Journal of Bioeconomics Aims and scope

Abstract

At the conclusion of 2017, to the dismay of journalists, pundits, and academics, large numbers of adolescents began consuming Tide Pods, a form of laundry detergent that is candy-like in appearance. This paper argues that purposeful consumption of laundry detergent may in fact be individually rational for adolescents. The consumption of Tide Pods may allow adolescents to successfully signal status in accordance with the Handicap Principle, which explains the beauty of a peacock’s tail and the practice of stotting by gazelles in the wild. The Handicap Principle is also a common explanation of adolescents’ willingness to engage in dangerous activities, like drug use. A subtext of the thesis of this paper is the veracity of rational choice explanations in unconventional contexts distant from its original applications.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Institutional subscriptions

Similar content being viewed by others

Notes

  1. https://www.youtube.com/watch?v=-DrC_PF_3Lg.

  2. The “preferences” of the genes in this context may remain constant, however, with the changes in preferences of the economic actor driven by psychological responses to changes in relative costs in the environment.

  3. Of lesser relevance here is a philosopher puzzle by Kavka (1983) related to the rationality of poison consumption.

  4. For an exposition of a very similar theory, that low socioeconomic status creates a cue for individuals to engage in risky behavior, can be found in Griskevicius et al. (2011). As long as the link between status seeking and risk remains, the arguments in this paper remain the same. The other exposition of the mechanism was emphasized because it appears to be more predominant in the literature.

  5. I should note that experimental evidence using mating priming has failed to replicate (Shanks et al. 2015), but this is one small piece of a core concept within the field of evolutionary psychology.

  6. Murphy (2016) argues that it is possible to interpret some forms of status seeking as irrational should there arise a disconnect between what people believe they are doing and what is ultimately status seeking behavior. In those examples forms of status seeking took on a veneer of morality. This disconnect may also arise in the Ice Bucket Challenge. It does not seem to be in play in explaining the Tide Pod Challenge.

  7. There are some very small additional benefits that I have not mentioned, such as serving the function of sorting partners. However, this may not be a social good, as assortive mating is one cause of income inequality (Greenwood et al. 2014). Social benefits accrued are likely smaller than they are in education, for one example, which has received lengthy criticism elsewhere for imposing deadweight loss via a similar mechanism (Caplan 2018). There is a more speculative benefit to the Tide Pod Challenge, which is that it may on net reduce laundry detergent capsule poisonings. Despite a galling 215 cases of teens being hospitalized for consuming Tide Pods in the first 4 months of 2018, the overall pattern is a reduction in the number of children facing such treatment. It may be that the ironic outcome of the Tide Pod Challenge has been to raise awareness and actually create greater net social benefits than the Ice Bucket Challenge (which has received some criticism, see Belluz 2014). Somewhat less provocatively, Collins et al. (2015) argue that conspicuous consumption played a role in developing modern rates of economic growth.

  8. See Boardman et al. (2006: 411) for a straightforward distillation of the results.

  9. This estimate is $1.47 million, which the authors find after adjusting for publication bias.

  10. In saying this, the model I have in mind is the Tide Pod Challenge as a “rite of passage” pooling equilibrium, as opposed to something done with any frequency.

References

  • Abad-Santos, A. (2018). Why people are (mostly) joking about eating Tide Pods. Vox, https://www.vox.com/2018/1/4/16841674/tide-pods-eating-meme-tide-pod-challenge

  • Alchian, A. (1950). Uncertainty, evolution, and economic theory. Journal of Political Economy, 58(3), 211–221.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • American Association of Poison Control Centers. (2018a). Laundry detergent packets (unit dose liquid) data. Retrieved October 4, 2018, from https://aapcc.s3.amazonaws.com/files/library/Laundry_Pack_Web_Data_through_3.2018.pdf.

  • American Association of Poison Control Centers. (2018b). Intentional exposures among teens to single-load laundry packets. Retrieved October 4, 2018, from http://www.aapcc.org/alerts/intentional-exposures-among-teens-single-load-laun/. This page was deleted but has been archived: https://web.archive.org/web/20180430235815/, http://www.aapcc.org/alerts/intentional-exposures-among-teens-single-load-laun/

  • Baker Jr, M. D., & Maner, J. K. (2008). Risk-taking as a situationally sensitive male mating strategy. Evolution and Human Behavior, 29, 391–395.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Baker Jr, M. D., & Maner, J. K. (2009). Male risk-taking as a context-sensitive signaling device. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 45(5), 1136–1139.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Basso, F., Robert-Demontrond, P., Hayek, M., Anton, J.-L., Nazarian, B., Roth, M., & Oullier, O. (2014). Why people drink shampoo? Food imitating products are fooling brains and endangering consumers for marketing purposes. PLoS ONE, 9(9), 1–17.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Becker, G. (1962). Irrational behavior and economic theory. Journal of Political Economy, 70(1), 1–13.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Becker, G., & Murphy, K. (1988). A theory of rational addiction. Journal of Political Economy, 96(4), 675–700.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Belluz, J. (2014). The truth about the ice bucket challenge: Viral memes shouldn’t dictate our charitable giving. Vox, https://www.vox.com/2014/8/20/6040435/als-ice-bucket-challenge-and-why-we-give-to-charity-donate

  • Bever, L. (2018). Teens are daring each other to eat Tide Pods. We don’t need to tell you that’s a bad idea. The Washington Post, https://www.washingtonpost.com/news/to-your-health/wp/2018/01/13/teens-are-daring-each-other-to-eat-tide-pods-we-dont-need-to-tell-you-thats-a-bad-idea/?utm_term=.7dae3d8c8955

  • Boardman, A. E., Greenberg, D. H., Vining, A. R., & Weimer, D. L. (2006). Cost-benefit analysis: Concepts and practice (3rd ed.). Upper Saddle River, NJ: Pearson Prentice Hall.

    Google Scholar 

  • Borkowska, B., & Pawlowski, B. (2014). Recreational drug use and fluctuating asymmetry: Testing the handicap principle. Evolutionary Psychology, 12(4), 769–782.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Byrnes, J. P., Miller, D. C., & Schafer, W. D. (1999). Gender differences in risk taking: A meta-analysis. Psychological Bulletin, 125(3), 367–383.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Caplan, Bryan. (2018). The case against education: Why the education system is a waste of time and money. Princeton: Princeton University Press.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Collins, J. (2017). Rationalizing the ‘irrational’. Behavioral Scientist, http://behavioralscientist.org/rationalizing-the-irrational/

  • Collins, J., Baer, B., & Weber, E. J. (2015). Sexual selection, conspicuous consumption and economic growth. Journal of Bioeconomics, 17(2), 186–209.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Consumer Reports News. (2013). As poisoning cases rise, a call for safer laundry pod packaging. https://www.consumerreports.org/cro/news/2013/03/as-poisoning-cases-rise-a-call-for-safer-laundry-pod-packaging/index.htm

  • Dahir, I. (2018). Let’s kick of 2018 with a warning from tide not to eat its laundry pacs. Buzzfeed, https://www.buzzfeed.com/ikrd/the-tide-pods-wont-get-you-high?utm_term=.vuZk6ZxeAK#.uvV1Q3KBgY

  • De Fraja, G. (2009). The origins of utility: Sexual selection and conspicuous consumption. Journal of Economic Behavior & Organization, 72(1), 51–69.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Diamond, Jared. (1992). The third chimpanzee: The evolution and future of the human animal. New York: HarperCollins.

    Google Scholar 

  • Durrant, Russell, Adamson, Simon, Todd, Fraser, & Selmman, Doug. (2009). Drug use and addiction: Evolutionary perspective. Australian and New Zealand Journal of Psychiatry, 43(11), 1049–1056.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Farthing, G. William. (2005). Attitudes toward heroic and nonheroic physical risk takers as mates and as friends. Evolution and Human Behavior, 26(2), 171–185.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Frank, R. H. (2005). Departures from rational choice: With and without regret. In Francesco Parisi & Vernon L. Smith (Eds.), The law and economics of irrational behavior. Stanford: Stanford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Frank, R. H. (2018). Why even tougher smoking regulations are justified. The New York Times, https://www.nytimes.com/2018/01/05/business/economy/why-even-tougher-regulations-on-smoking-are-justified.html

  • Grant-Alfieri, A., Schaechter, J., & Lipshultz, S. E. (2013). Ingesting and aspirating dry cinnamon by children and adolescents: the ‘cinnamon challenge’. Pediatrics, 131(5), 833–835.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Greenwood, J., Guner, N., Kocharkov, G., & Santos, C. (2014). Marry your like: Assortive mating and income inequality. American Economic Review, 105(4), 348–353.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Greitemeyer, T., Kastemuller, A., & Fischer, P. (2013). Romantic motives and risk-taking: An evolutionary approach. Journal of Risk Research, 16(1), 19–38.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Griskevicius, V., Tybur, J. M., Delton, A. W., & Robertson, T. E. (2011). The theory of mortality and socioeconomic status on risk and delayed rewards: A life history theory approach. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 100(6), 1015–1026.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Gstalater, M. (2018). YouTube removing videos of people eating Tide Pods. The Hill, http://thehill.com/policy/technology/369531-youtube-facebook-removing-videos-of-people-eating-tide-pods

  • Hagen, E. H., Roulette, C. J., & Sullivan, R. J. (2013). Explaining human recreational use of ‘pesticides’: The neurotoxin regulation model of substance use vs. the hijack model and implications for age and sex differences in drug consumption. Frontiers in Psychiatry, 4(Article 142), 1–21.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hoffer, S., Beradino, F., Smith, J., & Rubin, S. (1998). Economic values for evaluation of FAA investment and regulatory decisions. Washington, DC: Federal Aviation Administration.

    Google Scholar 

  • Huntington, S., Heppner, J., Vohra, R., Mallois, R., & Geller, R. J. (2014). Serious adverse effects from single-use detergent sacs: Report from a U.S. Statewide Poison Control System. Clinical Toxicology, 52, 220–225.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ioannidis, J. P. A., Stanley, T. D., & Doucouliagos, H. (2017). The power of bias in economics research. The Economic Journal, 127(605), F236–F265.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Karel, L. I., Handzel, M. C., & Rosini, J. M. (2015). Laundry detergent pod ingestion in 2 pediatric patients. Journal of Emergency Nursing, 41(1), 80–82.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kavka, G. S. (1983). The toxin puzzle. Analysis, 43(1), 33–36.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kelly, S., & Dunbar, R. I. M. (2001). Who dares, wins. Human Nature, 12(2), 89–105.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kenrick, D. T., & Griskevicius, V. (2013). The rational animal: How evolution made us smarter than we think. New York: Basic Books.

    Google Scholar 

  • KnowYourMeme. (2018). Tide POD challenge.” http://knowyourmeme.com/memes/tide-pod-challenge

  • Leeson, P. T. (2012). Ordeals. Journal of Law and Economics, 55(3), 691–714.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Leeson, P. T. (2017). WTF?! An economic tour of the weird. Stanford: Stanford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Leeson, P. T., & Coyne, C. J. (2012). Sassywood. Journal of Comparative Economics, 40, 608–620.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Levitt, S. D., & Venkatesh, S. A. (2000). An economic analysis of a drug-selling gang’s finances. Quarterly Journal of Economics, 115(3), 755–789.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Murphy, R. H. (2016). The willingness-to-pay for caplanian irrationality. Rationality and Society, 28(1), 52–82.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Popper, K. (1959). The logic of scientific discovery. London: Hutchinson Co.

    Google Scholar 

  • Redford, A. (2017). Don’t eat the brown acid: Induced ‘malnovation’ in drug markets. Review of Austrian Economics, 30(2), 215–233.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ridley, M. (1996). The origins of virtue: human instincts and the evolution of cooperation. New York: Penguin.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ronay, R., & von Hippel, W. (2010). The presence of an attractive woman elevates testosterone and physical risk taking in young men. Social Psychological and Personality Science, 1(1), 57–64.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Rubin, P. (2003). Folk economics. Southern Economic Journal, 70(1), 157–171.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Rubin, P., & Paul, C. W. (1979). An evolutionary model of taste for risk. Economic Inquiry, 17(4), 585–596.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Shanks, D. R., Vadillo, M. A., Riedel, B., Clymo, A., Govind, S., Hickin, N., Tamman, A. J. F., & Puhlmann, L. M. C. (2015). Romance, risk, and replication: can consumer choices and risk-taking be primed by mating motives? Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 144(6), e142–e158.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Sjogren, P. P., Skarda, D. E., & Park, A. H. (2016). Upper aerodigestive injuries from detergent ingestion in children. The Laryngoscope, 127(February), 509–512.

    Google Scholar 

  • Smith, E., Liebelt, E., & Nogueira, J. (2014). Laundry detergent pod ingestions: Is there a need for endoscopy? Journal of Medical Toxicology, 10(3), 286–291.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Steinberg, L. (2008). A social neuroscience perspective on adolescent risk-taking. Development Review, 28(1), 78–106.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Stigler, G., & Becker, G. (1977). De gustibus non est disputandum. American Economic Review, 67(2), 76–90.

    Google Scholar 

  • Stromberg, P. E., Burt, M. H., Rose, S. R., Cumpston, K. L., Emswiler, M. P., & Wills, B. K. (2015). Airway compromise in children exposed to single-use laundry detergent pods: A poison center observational case series. American Journal of Emergency Medicine, 33, 349–351.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Sundie, J. M., Kenrick, D. T., Griskevicius, V., Tybur, J. M., Vohs, K. D., & Beal, D. J. (2010). Peacocks, porsches, and thorstein veblen: Conspicuous consumption as a sexual signaling system. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 100(4), 664–680.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Tullock, G. (1962). The welfare costs of tariffs, monopolies, and theft. Economic Inquiry, 5(2), 224–232.

    Google Scholar 

  • Valdez, A. L., Casavant, M. J., Spiller, H. A., Chounthirath, T., Xiang, H., & Smith, G. A. (2014). Pediatric exposure to laundry pods. Pediatrics, 134(6), 1127–1135.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Veblen, T. (1899). The theory of the leisure class. Abingdon: Macmillan.

    Google Scholar 

  • Vincke, E. (2017). The deep rationality of dark consumption: Alcohol and cigarette use as sexual signaling. Dissertation, Ghent University.

  • von Mises, L. (1949). Human action. New Haven: Yale University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wilke, A., Hutchinson, J. M. C., Todd, P. M., & Kruger, D. J. (2006). Is risk taking used as a cue in mate choice? Evolutionary Psychology, 4, 367–393.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Wilson, M., & Daly, M. (1985). Competitiveness, risk taking, and violence: The young male syndrome. Ethology and Sociobiology, 6(1), 59–73.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Yin, S., Behrman, A., & Colvin, J. (2015). Laundry pack exposures in children 0-5 years evaluated at a single pediatric institution. The Journal of Emergency Medicine, 48(5), 566–572.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Zahavi, A. (1975). Mate selection—A selection for handicap. Journal of Theoretical Biology, 53(1), 205–214.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Zahavi, A., & Zahavi, A. (1997). The handicap principle: A missing piece of Darwin’s puzzle. Oxford: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Ryan H. Murphy.

Additional information

Publisher’s Note

Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Murphy, R.H. The rationality of literal Tide Pod consumption. J Bioecon 21, 111–122 (2019). https://doi.org/10.1007/s10818-019-09285-1

Download citation

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s10818-019-09285-1

Keywords

JEL Classification

Navigation