Skip to main content
Log in

How Adolescents with Autism Spectrum Disorder (ASD) Spontaneously Attend to Real-World Scenes: Use of a Change Blindness Paradigm

  • Original Paper
  • Published:
Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

Visual attention of adolescents with autism spectrum disorder (ASD) was assessed using a change blindness paradigm. Twenty-five adolescents with ASD aged 12–18 years and 25 matched typically developing (TD) adolescents viewed 36 pairs of digitized real-world images. Each pair of images was displayed in a ‘flicker paradigm’ whereby a particular item alternately appeared and disappeared. This item was either a central or a marginal detail of the scene. Change detection response times were measured and compared between groups. Marginal details were more difficult to detect than central details of the scenes in both groups, however, the response times of the ASD group were lower than the TD group. These results challenge the hypothesis of superior visual detection in ASD.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Fig. 1
Fig. 2
Fig. 3

Similar content being viewed by others

References

  • American Psychiatric Association. (2013). Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders: DSM 5. Washington: American Psychiatric Association.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Benson, V., Castelhano, M. S., Howard, P. L., Latif, N., & Rayner, K. (2016). Looking, seeing and believing in autism: Eye movements reveal how subtle cognitive processing differences impact in the social domain. Autism Research, 9(8), 879–887.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Burack, J. A., Joseph, S., Russo, N., Shore, D. I., Porporino, M., & Enns, J. T. (2009). Change detection in naturalistic pictures among children with autism. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 39(3), 471–479.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Dunn, L., & Dunn, D. (2007). Peabody picture vocabulary test. bloomington, MN: NCS pearson.

    Google Scholar 

  • Enns, J. T., & Trick, L. M. (2006). Four modes of selection. In Lifespan cognition: Mechanisms of change (pp. 43–56). Oxford: Oxford University Press.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Ferrer, E., Whitaker, K. J., Steele, J. S., Green, C. T., Wendelken, C., & Bunge, S. A. (2013). White matter maturation supports the development of reasoning ability through its influence on processing speed. Developmental Science, 16(6), 941–951.

    PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Fletcher-Watson, S., Findlay, J. M., Leekam, S. R., & Benson, V. (2008). Rapid detection of person information in a naturalistic scene. Perception, 37(4), 571–583.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Fletcher-Watson, S., Leekam, S., Turner, M., & Moxon, L. (2006). Do people with autistic spectrum disorder show normal selection for attention? Evidence from change blindness. British Journal of Psychology, 97(4), 537–554.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Fletcher-Watson, S., Leekam, S. R., Connolly, B., Collis, J. M., Findlay, J. M., McConachie, H., & Rodgers, J. (2012). Attenuation of change blindness in children with autism spectrum disorders. British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 30(3), 446–458.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Frith, U., & Happé, F. (1994). Autism: Beyond “theory of mind”. Cognition, 50(1–3), 115–132.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Happé, F., & Frith, U. (2006). The weak coherence account: Detail-focused cognitive style in autism spectrum disorders. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 36(1), 5–25.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Iarocci, G., Burack, J. A., Shore, D. I., Mottron, L., & Enns, J. T. (2006). Global–local visual processing in high functioning children with autism: Structural vs. implicit task biases. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 36(1), 117–129.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Jolliffe, T., & Baron-Cohen, S. (1997). Are people with autism and asperger syndrome faster than normal on the embedded figures test? Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 38(5), 527–534.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Kail, R. (1991). Developmental change in speed of processing during childhood and adolescence. Psychological Bulletin, 109(3), 490–501. doi:10.1037/0033-2909.109.3.490.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Kail, R. V., & Ferrer, E. (2007). Processing speed in childhood and adolescence: Longitudinal models for examining developmental change. Child Development, 78(6), 1760–1770.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Kaldy, Z., Giserman, I., Carter, A. S., & Blaser, E. (2016). The mechanisms underlying the ASD advantage in visual search. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 46(5), 1513–1527.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Keehn, B., Müller, R., & Townsend, J. (2013). Atypical attentional networks and the emergence of autism. Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews, 37(2), 164–183.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kevin O’Regan, J., Deubel, H., Clark, J. J., & Rensink, R. A. (2000). Picture changes during blinks: Looking without seeing and seeing without looking. Visual Cognition, 7(1–3), 191–211.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lavie, N. (2005). Distracted and confused? Selective attention under load. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 9(2), 75–82.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Lavie, N. (2010). Attention, distraction, and cognitive control under load. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 19(3), 143–148.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lavie, N., Hirst, A., de Fockert, J. W., Viding, E. (2004). Load theory of selective attention and cognitive control. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 133(3), 339–354.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lord, C., Rutter, M., DiLavore, P., Risi, S., Gotham, K., & Bishop, S. (2012). Autism diagnostic observation schedule, (ADOS-2) modules 1–4. Los Angeles: Western Psychological Services.

    Google Scholar 

  • Loth, E., Carlos Gómez, J., & Happé, F. (2008). Detecting changes in naturalistic scenes: Contextual inconsistency does not influence spontaneous attention in high-functioning people with autism spectrum disorder. Autism Research, 1(3), 179–188.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Mayes, S. D., & Calhoun, S. L. (2007). Learning, attention, writing, and processing speed in typical children and children with ADHD, autism, anxiety, depression, and oppositional-defiant disorder. Child Neuropsychology, 13(6), 469–493.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Mottron, L., Burack, J. A., Iarocci, G., Belleville, S., & Enns, J. T. (2003). Locally oriented perception with intact global processing among adolescents with high-functioning autism: Evidence from multiple paradigms. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 44(6), 904–913.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Mottron, L., Dawson, M., Soulieres, I., Hubert, B., & Burack, J. (2006). Enhanced perceptual functioning in autism: An update, and eight principles of autistic perception. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 36(1), 27–43.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • New, J. J., Schultz, R. T., Wolf, J., Niehaus, J. L., Klin, A., German, T. C., & Scholl, B. J. (2010). The scope of social attention deficits in autism: Prioritized orienting to people and animals in static natural scenes. Neuropsychologia, 48(1), 51–59.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • O’Riordan, M. A., Plaisted, K. C., Driver, J., & Baron-Cohen, S. (2001). Superior visual search in autism. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 27(3), 719–730. doi:10.1037//0096-1523.27.3.719.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Oliveras-Rentas, R. E., Kenworthy, L., Roberson, R. B., Martin, A., & Wallace, G. L. (2012). WISC-IV profile in high-functioning autism spectrum disorders: Impaired processing speed is associated with increased autism communication symptoms and decreased adaptive communication abilities. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 42(5), 655–664.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Pashler, H., Johnston, J. C., & Ruthruff, E. (2001). Attention and performance. Annual Review of Psychology, 52(1), 629–651.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Pashler, H. E. (1998). Psychology of attention. Cambridge: MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Raven, J. C. (1998). Raven’s progressive matrices. Oxford: Oxford Psychologists Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Remington, A., Swettenham, J., Campbell, R., & Coleman, M. (2009). Selective attention and perceptual load in autism spectrum disorder. Psychological Science, 20(11), 1388–1393. doi:10.1111/j.1467-9280.2009.02454.x.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Rensink, R. A. (2002). Change detection. Annual Review of Psychology, 53(1), 245–277.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Rensink, R. A., O’Regan, J. K., & Clark, J. J. (1997). To see or not to see: The need for attention to perceive changes in scenes. Psychological Science, 8(5), 368–373.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Riby, D. M., Brown, P. H., Jones, N., & Hanley, M. (2012). Brief report: Faces cause less distraction in autism. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 42(4), 634–639.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Rutter, M., Bailey, A., Lord, C., & Berument, S. (2003). Social communication questionnaire. Los Angeles: Western Psychological Services.

    Google Scholar 

  • Sakia, R. (1992). The box-cox transformation technique: A review. The Statistician, 169–178.

  • Shah, A., & Frith, U. (1993). Why do autistic individuals show superior performance on the block design task? Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 34(8), 1351–1364.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Sheth, B. R., Liu, J., Olagbaju, O., Varghese, L., Mansour, R., Reddoch, S., … Loveland, K. A. (2011). Detecting social and non-social changes in natural scenes: Performance of children with and without autism spectrum disorders and typical adults. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 41(4), 434–446.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Shore, D. I., Burack, J. A., Miller, D., Joseph, S., & Enns, J. T. (2006). The development of change detection. Developmental Science, 9(5), 490–497.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Shore, D. I., & Klein, R. M. (2000). The effects of scene inversion on change blindness. The Journal of General Psychology, 127(1), 27–43.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Simons, D. J., & Rensink, R. A. (2005). Change blindness: Past, present, and future. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 9(1), 16–20.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Smith, H., & Milne, E. (2009). Reduced change blindness suggests enhanced attention to detail in individuals with autism. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 50(3), 300–306.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Van Dillen, L. F., Heslenfeld, D. J., & Koole, S. L. (2009). Tuning down the emotional brain: An fMRI study of the effects of cognitive load on the processing of affective images. NeuroImage, 45(4), 1212–1219.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Wallace, G. L., Anderson, M., & Happé, F. (2009). Brief report: Information processing speed is intact in autism but not correlated with measured intelligence. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 39(5), 809–814.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Wallace, G. L., Kenworthy, L., Pugliese, C. E., Popal, H. S., White, E. I., Brodsky, E., & Martin, A. (2016). Real-world executive functions in adults with autism spectrum disorder: Profiles of impairment and associations with adaptive functioning and co-morbid anxiety and depression. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 46(3), 1071–1083.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgments

This work was supported in part by the PICS program (Ref: PICS07209) of the CNRS (National Center for Scientific Research, France) and by the Caesarea Edmond Benjamin De Rothschild Institute (Haifa, Israel). This work was also supported by Erasmus Mundus EMAIL II Program of the European Union.

Author Contributions

MH conceptualized and designed the study, recruited participants, carried out the initial analyses, drafted the initial manuscript, reviewed and revised the manuscript. OG conceptualized and designed the study, carried out the initial analyses, drafted the initial manuscript and reviewed and revised the manuscript. AA recruited participants, collected data and reviewed and revised the manuscript. All authors approved the final manuscript as submitted and agree to be accountable for all aspects of the work.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Michal Hochhauser.

Ethics declarations

Conflict of interest

The authors declare that they have no conflict of interest.

Ethical Approval

All procedures performed in studies involving human participants were in accordance with the ethical standards of the institutional and/or national research committee and with the 1964 Helsinki declaration and its later amendments or comparable ethical standards.

Informed Consent

Informed consent was obtained from all individual participants included in the study.

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Hochhauser, M., Aran, A. & Grynszpan, O. How Adolescents with Autism Spectrum Disorder (ASD) Spontaneously Attend to Real-World Scenes: Use of a Change Blindness Paradigm. J Autism Dev Disord 48, 502–510 (2018). https://doi.org/10.1007/s10803-017-3343-6

Download citation

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s10803-017-3343-6

Keywords

Navigation