Skip to main content
Log in

Forms of Aggression, Social-Psychological Adjustment, and Peer Victimization in a Japanese Sample: The Moderating Role of Positive and Negative Friendship Quality

  • Published:
Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

The purpose of these studies was to examine the frequency and stability of relational and physical aggression and their associations with social-psychological adjustment or peer victimization, and how friendships are involved in the relations between forms of aggression and peer victimization in Japanese children. The sample consisted of 452 (Study 1) and 138 (Study 2) children who were in the fourth and fifth grades. Results of Study 1 demonstrated that relational aggression was uniquely and more strongly associated with internalizing adjustment problems than physical aggression. Moreover, Study 2 revealed that relational aggression and physical aggression were stable over a 6-month period and the stability of relational aggression was reinforced by negative friendships (i.e., high levels of exclusivity and friend victimization). Further, the association between relational aggression and relative increases in relational victimization was attenuated by positive friendships (i.e., high levels of intimacy, companionship, and friendship satisfaction). Interestingly, friendships were unrelated to physical aggression and its relation to physical victimization. The age and gender of the children in the two studies were also examined. Cultural and developmental processes involving forms of aggression, friendships, social-psychological adjustment, and peer victimization were discussed.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Institutional subscriptions

Fig. 1
Fig. 2

Similar content being viewed by others

Notes

  1. Additional two-way interaction terms (i.e., physical aggression × gender, relational aggression × gender, physical aggression × grade, and relational aggression × grade) were entered in order to examine the role of gender and age in the association between relational aggression or physical aggression and social-psychological adjustment problems. We found that most interaction terms were not significant. Thus, these results were not included in final models. One exception was the significant relational aggression × gender interaction for externalizing adjustment problems. A follow-up mixed linear model revealed that the relation between relational aggression and externalizing adjustment problems was significant only for boys (B = 0.25, p < 0.001, B = −0.12, n.s., for boys and girls, respectively).

  2. Additional interaction terms (i.e., relational aggression × gender or grade, positive friendship × relational aggression × gender or grade, and negative friendship × relational aggression × gender or grade) were entered in order to examine the role of gender or age in the association between relational aggression and criterion variables and the role of gender and age in the moderating effect of friendship quality on these associations. We found that no interaction terms were significant and improved the overall model fit. Thus, the results were not included in final models.

  3. Additional interaction terms (i.e., physical aggression × gender or grade, positive friendship × physical aggression × gender or grade, and negative friendship × physical aggression × gender or grade) were entered in order to examine the role of gender and age in the association between physical aggression and criterion variables and the role of gender and age in the moderating effect of friendship quality on these associations. We found that most interaction terms were not significant. One exception was the significant positive friendship × physical aggression × grade interaction for physical aggression at time 2. However, the overall model fit did not improve by adding these interaction terms. This indicated that the quality of the additional model was questionable. Thus, the results were not included in final models.

References

  • Achenbach, T. M. (1991). Manual for the child behavior checklist 14–18 and 1991 profile. Burlington: University of Vermont Department of Psychiatry.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bandura, A. (1977). Social learning theory. Englewood Cliffs: Prentice Hall.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bjorkqvist, K. (1994). Sex differences in physical, verbal, and indirect aggression: a review of recent research. Sex Roles, 30, 177–188.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Card, N. A., Stucky, B. D., Sawalani, G. M., & Little, T. D. (2008). Direct and indirect aggression during childhood and adolescence: a meta-analytic review of gender differences, intercorrelations, and relations to maladjustment. Child Development, 79, 1467–8624.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Cohen, S., & Wills, T. A. (1985). Stress, social support, and the buffering hypothesis. Psychological Bulletin, 98, 310–357.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Coie, J. D., & Kupersmidt, J. B. (1983). A behavioral analysis of emerging social status in boys’ groups. Child Development, 54, 1400–1416.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Cote, S., Vaillancourt, T., Barker, T., Nagin, D., & Tremblay, R. E. (2007). The joint development of physical and indirect aggression: predictors of continuity and change during childhood. Development and Psychopathology, 19, 37–55.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Crick, N. R. (1996). The role of overt aggression, relational aggression, and prosocial behavior in the prediction of children’s future social adjustment. Child Development, 67, 2317–2327.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Crick, N. R., & Grotpeter, J. K. (1995). Relational aggression, gender, and social-psychological adjustment. Child Development, 66, 710–722.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Crick, N. R., & Zahn-Waxler, C. (2003). The development of psychopathology in females and males: current progress and future challenges. Development and Psychopathology, 15, 719–742.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Crick, N. R., Werner, N. E., Casas, J. F., O’Brien, K. M., Nelson, D. A., Grotpeter, J. K., et al. (1999). Childhood aggression and gender: A new look at an old problem. In D. Bernstein (Ed.), Nebraska symposium on motivation (pp. 75–141). Lincoln: University of Nebraska Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Crick, N. R., Ostrov, J. M., & Werner, N. E. (2006). A longitudinal study of relational aggression, physical aggression and children’s social-psychological adjustment problems. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 34, 131–142.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Crick, N. R., Ostrov, J. M., & Kawabata, Y. (2007). Relational aggression and gender: An overview. In D. J. Flannery, A. T. Vazsonyi, & I. D. Waldman (Eds.), The Cambridge handbook of violent behavior (pp. 245–259). New York: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Cullerton-Sen, C., & Crick, N. R. (2005). Understanding the effects of physical and relational victimization: the utility of multiple perspectives in predicting social-emotional adjustment. School Psychology Review, 34, 147–160.

    Google Scholar 

  • De Vos, G. A. (1996). Psychocultural continuities in Japanese social motivation. In D. W. Shwalb & B. J. Shwalb (Eds.), Japanese childrearing: Two generations of scholarship (pp. 44–84). New York: Guilford Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ministry of Education, Culture, Sports, Science, and Technology—Japan. (2004). A report concerning problematic behaviors of elementary school children. Retrieved from http://www.mext.go.jp/b_menu/houdou/16/08/04082302/all.pdf.

  • Enomoto, J. (1999). Socio-emotional development of friendship among adolescents: activities with friends and the feeling for friends. Japanese Journal of Educational Psychology, 47, 180–190.

    Google Scholar 

  • Enomoto, J. (2000). Friendship among adolescents: needs and their relation to emotions and activities. Japanese Journal of Educational Psychology, 48, 444–453.

    Google Scholar 

  • French, D. C., Jansen, E. A., & Pidada, S. (2002). United States and Indonesian children’s and adolescents’ reports of relational aggression by disliked peers. Child Development, 73, 1143–1150.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Galen, B. R., & Underwood, M. K. (1997). A developmental investigation of social aggression among children. Developmental Psychology, 33, 589–600.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Grotpeter, J. K., & Crick, N. R. (1996). Relational aggression, overt aggression, and friendship. Child Development, 67, 2328–2338.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Hart, C. H., Nelson, D. A., Robinson, C. C., Olsen, S. F., & McNeilly-Choque, M. K. (1998). Overt and relational aggression in Russian nursery-school-age children: parenting style and marital linkages. Developmental Psychology, 34, 687–697.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Hartup, W. W., & Stevens, N. (1997). Friendships and adaptation in the life course. Psychological Bulletin, 121, 355–370.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • House, J. S., Umberson, D., & Landis, K. R. (1988). Structures and processes of social support. Annual Review of Sociology, 14, 293–318.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Itani, T., Kanbayashi, Y., Nakata, Y., Kita, M., Fujii, H., Kuramoto, H., et al. (2001). Standardization of Japanese version of the child behavior checklist/4–18. Psychiatria et Neulrologia Paediatrica Japonica, 41, 243–252.

    Google Scholar 

  • Johnson, F. A. (1993). Dependency and Japanese socialization. New York: New York University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kawabata, Y., Crick, N. R., & Hamaguchi, Y. (2010). The role of culture in relational aggression: A link to social-psychological adjustment problems among Japanese and U.S. school-aged children. International Journal of Behavioral Development.

  • Koizumi, S. (1991). The standardization of children’s depression inventory. Syoni Hoken Kenkyu [The Journal of Child Health], 50, 717–721.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kovacs, M. (1985). The children’s depression inventory. Psychopharmacology Bulletin, 21, 995–998.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Maccoby, E. E. (1990). Gender and relationships. American Psychologist, 45, 513–520.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Markus, H. R., & Kitayama, S. (1991). Culture and the self: implications for cognition, emotion, and motivation. Psychological Review, 98, 224–253.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Morita, Y., Soeda, H., Soeda, K., & Taki, M. (1999). Japan. In P. K. Smith, Y. Morita, J. Junger-Tas, D. Olweus, R. Catalano, & P. Slee (Eds.), The nature of school bullying: A cross-national perspective (pp. 309–323). New York: Routledge.

    Google Scholar 

  • Murray-Close, D., Ostrov, J. M., & Crick, N. R. (2007). A short-term longitudinal study of growth of relational aggression during middle childhood: associations with gender, friendship intimacy, and internalizing problems. Development and Psychopathology, 19, 187–203.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Raudenbush, S. W., & Bryk, A. S. (2002). Hierarchical linear models: Applications and data analysis methods (2nd ed.). Newbury Park: Sage.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rose, A. J., & Rudolph, K. D. (2006). A review of sex differences in peer relationship processes: potential trade-offs for the emotional and behavioral development of girls and boys. Psychological Bulletin, 132, 98–131.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Rose, A. J., Swenson, L. P., & Waller, E. M. (2004). Overt and relational aggression and perceived popularity: developmental differences in concurrent and prospective relations. Developmental Psychology, 40, 378–387.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Rothbaum, F., Pott, M., Azuma, H., Miyake, K., & Weisz, J. (2000). The development of close relationships in Japan and the United States: paths of symbiotic harmony and generative tension. Child Development, 71, 1121–1142.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Sakai, A., & Yamasaki, K. (2004). Development of proactive and reactive aggression questionnaire for elementary school children. Japanese Journal of Psychology, 75, 254–261.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Sakurai, S. (1995). Educational social psychology on apathy. Tokyo: Kazama shobou.

    Google Scholar 

  • Shimizu, H. (2000). Japanese cultural psychology and empathic understanding: implications for academic and cultural psychology. Ethos, 28, 224–247.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Smith, P. K., Cowie, H., Olafsson, R. F., & Liefootghe, A. P. D. (2002). Definitions of bullying: a comparison of terms used, and age and gender differences in a fourteen-country international comparison. Child Development, 73, 1119–1133.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Vaillancourt, T., Brendgen, M., Boivin, M., & Tremblay, R. (2003). Longitudinal confirmatory factor analysis of indirect and physical aggression: evidence of two factors over time? Child Development, 74, 1628–1638.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Werner, N. E., & Crick, N. R. (2004). Peer relationship influences on the development of relational and physical aggression during middle childhood: the roles of peer rejection and association with aggressive friends. Social Development, 13, 495–513.

    Article  Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Yoshito Kawabata.

Additional information

This study was funded in part by the Institute of Child Development (ICD) through the second author. We thank the principals, teachers, parents and children for their participation. Portions of this paper were presented at the biennial meeting of the Society for Research in Child Development, Atlanta, GA, April, 2005.

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Cite this article

Kawabata, Y., Crick, N.R. & Hamaguchi, Y. Forms of Aggression, Social-Psychological Adjustment, and Peer Victimization in a Japanese Sample: The Moderating Role of Positive and Negative Friendship Quality. J Abnorm Child Psychol 38, 471–484 (2010). https://doi.org/10.1007/s10802-010-9386-1

Download citation

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s10802-010-9386-1

Keywords

Navigation