Skip to main content
Log in

Most and Least: Differences in Integer Comparisons Based on Temperature Comparison Language

  • Published:
International Journal of Science and Mathematics Education Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

The language involved in de-contextualized integer comparisons poses challenges, as students may interpret “most” based on absolute values rather than on order. Using the context of temperature, we explored how students’ integer value comparisons differed based on question phrasing (which temperature is hottest, most hot, least hot, coldest, most cold, least cold) and on numbers presented (positive, negative, mixed). Participants included 88 second graders and 70 fourth graders from a rural school district in the Midwestern USA, and each student solved 36 integer comparisons. For comparisons with positive number choices, students had more difficulty with “coldest” than “hottest”; however, the results were reversed for comparisons with only negative number choices. When working with mixed comparisons, students often chose the least of the cold as opposed to the least cold, suggesting that they saw hot and cold as categorical opposites rather than opposites on a continuum, with zero as a boundary.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Fig. 1

Similar content being viewed by others

References

  • Adetula, L. O. (1990). Language factor: Does it affect children’s performance on word problems? Educational Studies in Mathematics, 21(4), 351–365. https://doi.org/10.1007/BF00304263

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ball, D. L. (1993). With an eye on the mathematical horizon: Dilemmas of teaching elementary school mathematics. The Elementary School Journal, 93(4), 373–397.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Barner, D., & Snedeker, J. (2008). Compositionality and statistics in adjective acquisition: 4-year-olds interpret tall and short based on the size distributions of novel noun referents. Child Development, 79(3), 594–608. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-8624.2008.01145.x

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bell, A. (1984). Short and long term learning—Experiments in diagnostic teaching design. In B. Southwell (Ed.), Proceedings of the Eighth International Conference for the Psychology of Mathematics Education (pp. 55–62). Sydney, Australia: International Group for the Psychology of Mathematics Education.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bofferding, L. (2014). Negative integer understanding: Characterizing first graders’ mental models. Journal for Research in Mathematics Education, 45(2), 194–245. https://doi.org/10.5951/jresematheduc.45.2.0194.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bofferding, L. & Hoffman, A. (2015). Comparing negative integers: Issues of language. In K. Beswick, T. Muir, & J. Wells (Eds.), Proceedings of the 39th Conference of the International Group for the Psychology of Mathematics Education (Vol. 1, p. 150). Hobart, Australia: PME.

  • Case, R. (1996). Introduction: Reconceptualizing the nature of children’s conceptual structures and their development in middle childhood. Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development, 61(1–2), 1–26.

    Google Scholar 

  • Cheshire, J. (1998). Double negatives are illogical. In L. Bauer & P. Trudgill (Eds.), Language myths (pp. 113–122). New York, NY: Penguin Putnam, Inc..

    Google Scholar 

  • Clark, E. V. (1971). On the acquisition of the meaning of before and after. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 10(3), 266–275. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0022-5371(71)80054-3

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Clements, D. H., & Sarama, J. (2014). Learning and teaching early math: The learning trajectories approach (2nd ed.). New York, NY: Routledge.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Donaldson, M., & Balfour, G. (1968). Less is more: A study of language comprehension in children. British Journal of Psychology, 59(4), 461–471. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.2044-8295.1968.tb01163.x

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Dougherty, B. J. (2010). Developing essential understanding of number and numeration for teaching mathematics in prekindergarten–grade 2. Reston, VA: National Council of Teachers of Mathematics.

    Google Scholar 

  • Fuson, K. C., Carroll, W. M., & Landis, J. (1996). Level in conceptualizing and solving addition and subtraction compare problems. Cognition and Instruction, 14(3), 345–371. https://doi.org/10.1207/s1532690xci1403_3

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Gobbo, C., & Agnoli, F. (1985). Comprehension of two types of negative comparisons in children. Journal of Psycholinguistic Research, 14(3), 301–316. https://doi.org/10.1007/BF01068088

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Griffiths, J. A., Shantz, C. A., & Sigel, I. E. (1967). A methodological problem in conservation studies: The use of relational terms. Child Development, 38(3), 841–848. https://doi.org/10.2307/1127261

    Google Scholar 

  • Klatzky, R. L., Clark, E. V., & Macken, M. (1973). Asymmetries in the acquisition of polar adjectives: Linguistic or conceptual? Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 16(1), 32–46. https://doi.org/10.1016/0022-0965(73)90060-X

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lean, G. A., Clements, M. A., & Del Campo, G. (1990). Linguistic and pedagogical factors affecting children’s understanding of word problems: A comparative study. Educational Studies in Mathematics, 21(2), 165–191. https://doi.org/10.1007/BF00304900

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Murray, J. C. (1985). Children’s informal conceptions of integer arithmetic. In L. Streefland (Ed.), Proceedings of the Ninth Annual Conference of the International Group for the Psychology of Mathematics Education (Vol. 1, pp. 147–153). Noordwijkerhout, The Netherlands: International Group for the Psychology of Mathematics Education.

    Google Scholar 

  • Murray, P. L., & Mayer, R. E. (1988). Preschool children’s judgments of number magnitude. Journal of Educational Psychology, 80(2), 206–209. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-0663.80.2.206

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • National Council of Teachers of Mathematics (2000). Principles and standards for school mathematics. Retrieved from http://www.nctm.org/flipbooks/standards/pssm/index.html.

  • National Governors Association Center for Best Practices & Council of Chief State School Officers (2010). Common core state standards for mathematics. Retrieved from http://www.corestandards.org/Math/Content/K/introduction

  • National Research Council (2009). Mathematics learning in early childhood: Paths toward excellence and equity. Committee on Early Childhood Mathematics. In C.T. Cross, T.A. Woods & H. Schweingruber (Eds.), Center for Education, Division of Behavioral and Social Sciences and Education. Washington, DC: The National Academies Press.

  • Peled, I., Mukhoadhyay, S., & Resnick, L. (1989). Formal and informal sources of mental models for negative numbers. In G. Vergnaud, J. Rogalski, & M. Artique (Eds.), The international group for the psychology of mathematics education (Vol. 3, pp. 106–110). Paris: International Group for the Psychology of Mathematics Education.

    Google Scholar 

  • Pratt, D., & Simpson, A. (2004). McDonald’s vs Father Christmas. Australian Primary Mathematics Classroom, 9(3), 4–9.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ryalls, B. O. (2000). Dimensional adjectives: Factors affecting children’s ability to compare objects using novel words. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 76(1), 26–49. https://doi.org/10.1006/jecp.1999.2537

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Schwartz, B. B., Kohn, A. S., & Resnick, L. B. (1993-1994). Positives about negatives: A case study of an intermediate model for signed numbers. The Journal of the Learning Sciences, 3(1), 37–92.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Shire, B., & Durkin, K. (1989). Junior school children’s responses to conflict between the spatial and numerical meanings of ‘up’ and ‘down’. Educational Psychology, 9(2), 141–147. https://doi.org/10.1080/0144341890090206

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Siegler, R. S., & Robinson, M. (1982). The development of numerical understandings. In H. W. Reese & L. P. Lisitt (Eds.), Advances in child development and behavior (Vol. 16, pp. 242–312). New York, NY: Academic Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Smith, L. B., Rattermann, M. J., & Sera, M. (1988). “Higher” and “lower”: Comparative and categorical interpretations by children. Cognitive Development, 3(4), 341–357.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Sophian, C. (1987). Early developments in children’s use of counting to solve quantitative problems. Cognition and Instruction, 4(2), 61–90.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Stephan, M., & Akyuz, D. (2012). A proposed instructional theory for integer addition and subtraction. Journal for Research in Mathematics Education, 43(4), 428–464. https://doi.org/10.5951/jresematheduc.43.4.0428

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Swanson, P. E. (2010). The intersection of language and mathematics. Mathematics Teaching in the Middle School, 15(9), 516–523.

    Google Scholar 

  • Vamvakoussi, X., & Vosnaidou, S. (2012). Bridging the gap between the dense and the discrete: The number line and the “rubber line” bridging analogy. Mathematical Thinking and Learning, 14(4), 265–284. https://doi.org/10.1080/10986065.2012.717378

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Varma, S., & Schwartz, D. L. (2011). The mental representation of integers: An abstract-to-concrete shift in the understanding of mathematical concepts. Cognition, 121, 363–385. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cognition.2011.08.005

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Verschaffel, L. (1994). Using retelling data to study elementary school children’s representations and solutions of compare problems. Journal for Research in Mathematics Education, 25(2), 141–165. https://doi.org/10.2307/749506

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Vosnaidou, S., & Brewer, W. F. (1992). Mental models of the earth: A study of conceptual change in childhood. Cognitive Psychology, 24(4), 535–585. https://doi.org/10.1016/0010-0285(92)90018-W

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Vosniadou, S., Vamvakoussi, X., & Skopeliti, I. (2008). The framework theory approach to the problem of conceptual change. In S. Vosniadou (Ed.), International handbook of research on conceptual change (pp. 3–34). New York, NY: Routledge.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wessman-Enzinger, N. M., & Mooney, E. S. (2014). Making sense of integers through storytelling. Mathematics Teaching in the Middle School, 20(4), 202–205. https://doi.org/10.5951/mathteacmiddscho.20.4.0202

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Whitacre, I., Azuz, B., Lamb, L. L. C., Bishop, J., Schappelle, B. P., & Philipp, R. A. (2017). Integer comparisons across the grades: Students’ justifications and ways of reasoning. The Journal of Mathematical Behavior, 45, 47–62. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jmathb.2016.11.001

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Whitacre, I., Bishop, J. P., Philipp, R. A., Lamb, L. L., & Schappelle, B. P. (2015). Dollars & sense: Students’ integer perspectives. Mathematics Teaching in the Middle School, 20(2), 84–89. https://doi.org/10.5951/mathteacmiddscho.20.2.0084

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Widjaja, W., Stacey, K., & Steinle, V. (2011). Locating negative decimals on the number line: Insights into the thinking of pre-service primary teachers. The Journal of Mathematical Behavior, 30, 80–91. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jmathb.2010.11.004

    Article  Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgements

This research was supported by NSF CAREER award DRL-1350281. The authors would like to thank the schools, teachers, and students involved in the research for their participation and support. The authors give thanks especially to Mahtob Aqazade for her review of the paper.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Laura Bofferding.

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Bofferding, L., Farmer, S. Most and Least: Differences in Integer Comparisons Based on Temperature Comparison Language. Int J of Sci and Math Educ 17, 545–563 (2019). https://doi.org/10.1007/s10763-018-9880-4

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s10763-018-9880-4

Keywords

Navigation