Skip to main content
Log in

Variability of death penalty attitude in China: an empirical test of the Marshall hypotheses

  • Published:
Crime, Law and Social Change Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

Though empirical studies of the Marshall hypotheses are rich, few examined the hypotheses in non-US nations. Based on a sample of 1077 students and a quasi-experimental design, this study tests the Marshall hypotheses in China. Except the control group, three intervention essays (on ‘international trend’, ‘wrongful conviction’, and ‘deterrence’) were provided to three experimental groups and students’ opinions were surveyed afterwards on capital punishment overall and six specific capital offenses. The results showed that the majority of Chinese students favored capital punishment and the wrongful conviction essay helped significantly reduce students’ support in the overall death penalty opinion, consistent with the Marshall hypotheses. Nevertheless, the international trend and deterrence essays boosted students’ support when opinions on specific capital offenses were surveyed, producing a counter-effect. Consistent with the hypotheses, students with a retribution belief were more likely to favor capital punishment and less likely to be swayed by essay interventions.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Institutional subscriptions

Similar content being viewed by others

Notes

  1. A few of these studies did not test the effect of information on one’s overall opinion toward the death penalty, but on other things such as reasons for and factors associated with one’s support of capital punishment (e.g., [19, 20]) and one’s belief in fair application of capital punishment [21]. Careful interpretation is cautioned, though they can be viewed as general tests of the Marshall hypotheses.

  2. All Chinese universities have an official University Student Organization (xuesheng hui) who is in charge of student affairs and is often much more powerful and influential compared to Western counterparts.

  3. Normal universities in China (which aim to train future teachers at different levels) often have a high ratio of female students. We are informed that the particular Normal University of our study has a male-female ratio of 1:2.

  4. Studies of criminal justice and criminology in China are mainly covered in two separate fields, in law school under the study of criminal law and criminal procedure law and in universities and colleges specialized in policing.

  5. One other item measured one’s property crime experience in the last 5 years. We tested it in our MLR models, and it was not significant in any of them.

  6. Given the fact that our dependent variables are ordinal level variables, we initially ran ordinal regressions. Nevertheless, in four of the seven models (on overall opinion, drug trafficking, other violent crimes, and corruption), the assumption of parallelism was violated at the .05 significance level. As a result, we opted for MLRs. The results of ordinal regressions (available upon request) showed that none of the intervention groups had a significant independent effect upon students’ death penalty opinions. Results on demographic and control variables largely corroborated with the MLR results in Table 5.

References

  1. Harris, P. (1986). Over-simplification and error in public opinion surveys on capital punishment. Justice Quarterly, 3(4), 429–455.

    Google Scholar 

  2. Bohm, R. (1987). American death penalty attitudes: A critical examination of recent evidence. Criminal Justice and Behavior, 14(3), 380–396.

    Google Scholar 

  3. Bohm, R. (1992). Retribution and capital punishment: Toward a better understanding of death penalty opinion. Journal of Criminal Justice, 20(3), 227–236.

    Google Scholar 

  4. Warr, M., & Stafford, M. (1984). Public goals of punishment and support for the death penalty. Journal of Research in Crime and Delinquency, 21(2), 95–111.

    Google Scholar 

  5. Britt, C. (1998). Race, religion, and support for the death penalty: A research note. Justice Quarterly, 15(1), 175–191.

    Google Scholar 

  6. Tyler, T., & Weber, R. (1982). Support for the death penalty: Instrumental response to crime, or symbolic attitude? Law and Society Review, 17(1), 21–46.

    Google Scholar 

  7. Brace, P., & Boyea, B. (2008). State public opinion, the death penalty, and the practice of electing judges. American Journal of Political Science, 52(2), 360–372.

    Google Scholar 

  8. Ellsworth, P. C., & Ross, L. (1983). Public opinion and capital punishment: A close examination of the views of abolitionists and retentionists. Crime & Delinquency, 29(1), 116–169.

    Google Scholar 

  9. Jones, P. (1994). It’s not what you ask, it is the way that you ask it: Question form and public opinion on the death penalty. The Prison Journal, 73(1), 32–50.

    Google Scholar 

  10. Lu, J. (2009). On the application of the death penalty and public opinion. Chinese Sociology and Anthropology, 41(4), 66–79.

    Google Scholar 

  11. Sato, M. (2014). The death penalty in Japan: Will the public tolerate abolition? Wiesbaden: Springer VS.

  12. Vidmar, N., & Dittenhoffer, T. (1981). Informed public opinion and death penalty attitudes. Canadian Journal of Criminology, 23, 43–56.

    Google Scholar 

  13. Johnson, D. T., & Miao, M. (2016). Chinese capital punishment in comparative perspective. In B. Liang & H. Lu (Eds.), The death penalty in China: Policy, practice and reform (pp. 300–326). New York: Columbia University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  14. Zhang, N. (2016). Crimes of counterrevolution and politicized use of the death penalty during the Mao era. In B. Liang & H. Lu (Eds.), The death penalty in China: Policy, practice and reform (pp. 62–96). New York: Columbia University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  15. Liang, B., & Lu, H. (eds.). The death penalty in China: Policy, practice and reform. New York: Columbia University Press.

  16. Liang, B. (2014). Internet development and its influences on legal reform in China. China: An International Journal, 12(3), 27–54.

    Google Scholar 

  17. Sarat, A., & Vidmar, N. (1976). Public opinion, the death penalty, and the eighth amendment: Testing the Marshall hypothesis. Wisconsin Law Review, 17, 171–206.

    Google Scholar 

  18. LaChappelle, L. (2014). Capital punishment in the era of globalization: A partial test of the Marshall hypothesis among college students. American Journal of Criminal Justice, 39, 839–854.

    Google Scholar 

  19. Bohm, R., & Vogel, R. E. (1994). A comparison of factors associated with uninformed and informed death penalty opinions. Journal of Criminal Justice, 22(2), 125–143.

    Google Scholar 

  20. Bohm, R., Clark, L. J., & Aveni, A. F. (1990). The influence of knowledge on reasons for death penalty opinions: An experimental test. Justice Quarterly, 7(1), 175–188.

    Google Scholar 

  21. Murray, G. (2003). Raising considerations: Public opinion and the fair application of the death penalty. Social Science Quarterly, 84(4), 753–770.

    Google Scholar 

  22. Bohm, R. (1989). The effects of classroom instruction and discussion on death penalty opinions: A teaching note. Journal of Criminal Justice, 17(2), 123–131.

    Google Scholar 

  23. Bohm, R., & Vogel, R. E. (1991). Education experiences and death penalty opinions: Stimuli that produce changes. Journal of Criminal Justice Education, 2(1), 69–80.

    Google Scholar 

  24. Clarke, A. W., Lambert, E., & Whitt, L. A. (2001). Executing the innocent: The next step in the Marshall hypotheses. New York University Review of Law and Social Change, 26(3), 309–345.

    Google Scholar 

  25. Lambert, E., & Clarke, A. (2001). The impact of information on an individual’s support of the death penalty: A partial test of the Marshall hypothesis among college students. Criminal Justice Policy Review, 12(3), 215–234.

    Google Scholar 

  26. Lambert, E. G., Camp, S. D., Clarke, A., & Jiang, S. (2011). The impact of information on death penalty support, revisited. Crime & Delinquency, 57(4), 572–599.

    Google Scholar 

  27. Mitchell, A. D. (2006). The effect of the Marshall hypothesis on attitudes toward the death penalty. Race, Gender & Class, 13, 221–247.

    Google Scholar 

  28. Sandys, M. (1995). Attitudinal change among students in a capital punishment class: It may be possible. American Journal of Criminal Justice, 20(1), 37–55.

    Google Scholar 

  29. Bohm, R. (1990). Death penalty opinions: Effects of a classroom experience and public commitment. Sociological Inquiry, 60(3), 285–297.

    Google Scholar 

  30. Cox, A. K. (2013). Student death penalty attitudes: Does new information matter? Journal of Criminal Justice Education, 24(4), 443–460.

    Google Scholar 

  31. Falco, D. L., & Freiburger, T. L. (2011). Public opinion and the death penalty: A qualitative approach. The Qualitative Report, 16(3), 830–847.

    Google Scholar 

  32. Kennedy-Kollar, D., & Mandery, E. J. (2010). Testing the Marshall hypothesis and its antithesis: The effect of biased information on death penalty opinion. Criminal Justice Studies, 23(1), 65–83.

    Google Scholar 

  33. Vollum, S., & Buffington-Vollum, J. (2010). An examination of social-psychological factors and support for the death penalty: Attribution, moral disengagement, and the value-expressive function of attitudes. American Journal of Criminal Justice, 35, 15–36.

    Google Scholar 

  34. Vollum, S., Mallicoat, S., & Buffington-Vollum, J. (2009). Death penalty attitudes in an increasingly critical climate: Value-expressive support and attitude mutability. The Southwest Journal of Criminal Justice, 5(3), 221–242.

    Google Scholar 

  35. Lord, C. G., Ross, L., & Lepper, M. R. (1979). Biased assimilation and attitude polarization: The effects of prior theories on subsequently considered evidence. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 37(11), 2098–2109.

    Google Scholar 

  36. Bohm, R., Clark, L. J., & Aveni, A. F. (1991). Knowledge and death penalty opinion: A test of the Marshall hypotheses. Journal of Research in Crime and Delinquency, 28(3), 360–387.

    Google Scholar 

  37. Cochran, J. K., & Chamlin, M. B. (2005). Can information change public opinion? Another test of the Marshall hypotheses. Journal of Criminal Justice, 33(6), 573–584.

    Google Scholar 

  38. Cochran, J. K., Sanders, B., & Chamlin, M. B. (2006). Profiles in change: An alternative look at the Marshall hypotheses. Journal of Criminal Justice Education, 17(2), 205–226.

    Google Scholar 

  39. Mallicoat, S. L., & Brown, G. C. (2008). The impact of race and ethnicity on student opinions of capital punishment. Journal of Ethnicity in Criminal Justice, 6(4), 255–280.

    Google Scholar 

  40. Michel, C., & Cochran, J. K. (2011). The effects of information on change in death penalty support: Race- and gender-specific extensions of the Marshall hypotheses. Journal of Ethnicity in Criminal Justice, 9(4), 291–313.

    Google Scholar 

  41. Wright, H., Bohm, R., & Jamieson, K. M. (1995). A comparison of uninformed and informed death penalty opinions: A replication and expansion. American Journal of Criminal Justice, 20(1), 57–87.

    Google Scholar 

  42. Lee, G. M., Bohm, R., & Pazzani, L. M. (2014). Knowledge and death penalty opinion: The Marshall hypotheses revisited. American Journal of Criminal Justice, 39, 642–659.

    Google Scholar 

  43. Bohm, R., & Vogel, B. L. (2004). More than ten years after: The long-term stability of informed death penalty opinions. Journal of Criminal Justice, 32(4), 307–327.

    Google Scholar 

  44. Bohm, R., Vogel, R. E., & Maistro, A. A. (1993). Knowledge and death penalty opinions: A panel study. Journal of Criminal Justice, 21(1), 29–45.

    Google Scholar 

  45. Wang, Y. (2015). Beyond revenge: Public opinion, court and judicial control of the death penalty. Jilin University Journal (Social Sciences Edition), 55(4), 66–77.

    Google Scholar 

  46. Xiao, X. (2009). The interaction between public opinion and death penalty application in the context of China. Journal of Shandong University of Science and Technology (Social Science Edition), 11(1), 41–46.

    Google Scholar 

  47. Zhang, N. (2010). Public opinion and the death penalty debate in China. China Perspectives, 1, 85–96.

    Google Scholar 

  48. Fu, H. (2016). Between deference and defiance: Courts and penal populism in Chinese capital cases. In B. Liang & H. Lu (Eds.), The death penalty in China: Policy, practice and reform (pp. 274–299). New York: Columbia University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  49. Belkin, I. (2017). Justice in the PRC: How the Chinese community party has struggled with managing public opinion and the administration of criminal justice in the internet age. In F. Flora Sapio, S. Trevaskes, S. Biddulph, & E. Nesossi (Eds.), Justice: The China experience (pp. 195–228). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  50. Miao, M. (2013). Capital punishment in China: A populist instrument of social governance. Theoretical Criminology, 17(2), 233–250.

    Google Scholar 

  51. Tang, Y., & Wang, M. (2007). On public opinion and judicial control of capital punishment. In Li Jie et al. (Eds.), Criminal Law Problems in Harmonious Society (863–870). Beijing: Publishing House of People’s Public Security University of China.

  52. Zuo, J. (2009). The influence of public opinion on the application of the death penalty. Chinese Sociology and Anthropology, 41(4), 80–88.

    Google Scholar 

  53. Jiang, S. (2016). Public opinion and the death penalty. In B. Liang & H. Lu (Eds.), The death penalty in China: Policy, practice and reform (pp. 247–273). New York: Columbia University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  54. Zhao, J. (2015). Public opinion on the abolition of the death penalty: Measurement of the idea about inculpation of the crime of organizing prostitution. Chinese Journal of Law, 2, 26–41.

    Google Scholar 

  55. Cao, L., & Cullen, F. (2001). Thinking about crime and control: A comparative study of Chinese and American ideology. International Criminal Justice Review, 11(1), 58–81.

    Google Scholar 

  56. Jiang, S., Lambert, E., Wang, J., Saito, T., & Pilot, R. (2010). Death penalty views in China, Japan and the U.S.: An empirical comparison. Journal of Criminal Justice, 38(5), 862–869.

    Google Scholar 

  57. Qi, S., & Oberwittler, D. (2009). On the road to the rule of law: Crime, crime control, and public opinion in China. European Journal of Criminal Police and Research, 15(1–2), 137–157.

    Google Scholar 

  58. Wu, Y., Sun, I., & Wu, Z. (2011). Support for the death penalty: Chinese and American college students compared. Punishment and Society, 13, 354–376.

    Google Scholar 

  59. Jiang, S., Lambert, E., & Wang, J. (2007). Capital punishment views in China and the United States: A preliminary study among college students. International Journal of Offender Therapy and Comparative Criminology, 51(1), 84–97.

    Google Scholar 

  60. Liang, B., Lu, H., Miethe, T. D., & Zhang, L. (2006). Sources of variation in pro-death penalty attitudes in China: An exploratory study of Chinese students at home and abroad. British Journal of Criminology, 46(1), 119–130.

    Google Scholar 

  61. Oberwittler, D., & Qi, S. (2009). Public opinion on the death penalty in China: Results from a general population survey conducted in three provinces in 2007/08 (Forschung Aktuell/research in brief 41). Freiburg: Max Planck Institute for Foreign and International Criminal Law.

    Google Scholar 

  62. Zhang, H., & He, X. (2011). Survey report on the death penalty. Presentday Law Science, 9(4), 59–68.

    Google Scholar 

  63. He, J. (2016). Back from the dead: Wrongful convictions and criminal justice in China. Honolulu: University of Hawai’i Press.

    Google Scholar 

  64. Jia, Y. (2005). Death penalty views among college students in China: An empirical study. Legal Science Review (Faxue Pinglun), 3, 20–33.

  65. Kuang, L., Zeng, Y., Li, K., Liu, S., Chen, M., Dai, C., & Zhang, X. (2010). A survey on criminal justice professionals’ attitudes toward the death penalty. In H. Mo (Ed.), A Survey Report on the Death Penalty in China (pp. 63–224). Taibei: Yuanzhao Press.

    Google Scholar 

  66. Hovland, C. I., Harvey, O. J., & Kelley, H. H. (1953). Communication and persuasion. New Haven: Yale University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  67. Liu, J., & Liang, B. (2018) A case study of Chinese netizens’ opinions on capital punishment: Diversity, rationale, and interaction. Modern China. https://doi.org/10.1177/0097700418819833.

    Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Bin Liang.

Additional information

Publisher’s note

Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

Appendix: Intervention Essays

Appendix: Intervention Essays

Essay I: International Trend of the Death Penalty

Abolition of the death penalty is a major trend of the intentional community. Since 1990, the pace of death penalty abolition has been accelerated significantly and on average three nations abolished the death penalty in each year. By 2015, 140 countries of the world (more than two thirds of all nations) have abolished the death penalty either in law or in legal practice. Among developed countries, only the United States and Japan retain the death penalty, while the Europe Union, Canada and Australian countries all abolished their practices.

China was considered the largest user of the death penalty by the international community. Thousands of people are sentenced to death and executed every year. Besides China, countries with the most executions are Iran, Pakistan, Saudi Arabia and the United States. According to statistics reported by the Amnesty International, at least 1634 people were executed in 25 countries in 2015 (note: this number does not include China).

There are 46 capital offenses in the current Criminal Law of China (amended in 2015). Besides homicide and violent crimes, crimes that do not cause people’s death such as certain property crimes, economic crimes and corruption crimes can also be eligible for the death penalty. In comparison, federal laws of the United States contain 41 capital offenses, most of which are related to homicide and offenses that cause people’s death (such as deaths resulted from kidnapping, drug trafficking and aircraft hijacking). Only three offenses without human death are eligible for the death penalty in the United States, including treason, espionage and mailing of injurious articles with intent to kill. However in real legal practice, these three capital crimes are rarely applied and therefore exist in names only. In the new twenty-first century, death penalty executions in the United States experienced a gradual decline. In 2016, only 20 people were executed, which is the lowest number of execution since its peak in 1998 (when 98 people were executed).

In the process of death penalty abolition, international organizations played a prominent role. The Universal Declaration of Human Rights announced by the United Nations in 1948 proposed the concept of ‘the right to life’ for the first time. In 1966 the International Covenant on Civil and Political Rights (ICCPR) adopted by the United Nations General Assembly demanded that capital punishment, if retained, shall be imposed only for “the most serious crimes”, and encouraged eventual abolition of the death penalty by all nations. In 1989, the Second Optional Protocol to the ICCPR aiming at the abolition of the death penalty commits its members to the abolition of the death penalty within their borders except “in time of war pursuant to a conviction for a most serious crime of a military nature committed during wartime”. In 1997 the United Nations Commission on Human Rights adopted a resolution on the death penalty calling upon all countries that retain the death penalty to suspend executions, restrict the number of capital offenses, and timely make transparent information about one’s death penalty executions. In 2010, a resolution to establish a moratorium on the use of the death penalty by United Nations Human Rights Commission received 107 votes in favor, 38 against and 36 abstentions. The resolution gained the most number of votes in favor since its first proposal in 2007, which reconfirmed the world trend to establish a moratorium on the use of the death penalty.

All of the above indicate that the abolition of the death penalty has become a primary focus of international public law. Overemphasis on one’s self-determination and on one’s special national conditions while ignoring the trend of death penalty abolition under the background of international human rights movements does not bode well with China’s position as the world second largest economic entity. It could also create realistic problems in China’s legal practice (such as international judicial cooperation and extradition agreements). For example, Lai Changxing, the primary culprit of the Xiamen Yuanhua smuggling case in the 1990’s escaped to Canada after the exposure of his crimes. From 2000, the Chinese government had been negotiating with the Canadian government and asking for Lai’s extradition. However the Canadian government refused to consider the negotiation unless China promised not to death-sentence and execute Lai. Only until 2011 was Lai extradited to China and subsequently sentenced to life imprisonment in 2012. In recent years, judicial reforms in China (such as reduction of the number of capital offenses, restriction on eligibilities of capital offenders, and increasing procedural justice and transparency) further indicate that China is moving closer to the requirements of international standards. The number of capital offenses has been reduced from 68 in 1997 to 55 in 2011, and again to 46 in 2015.

Essay II: Wrongful Convictions and the Death Penalty

Disposition and prevention of wrongful convictions could be viewed as a litmus test to the criminal justice system of a nation. As early as 1747, Voltaire wrote, “it is far better to let a guilty man walk free than to have an innocent man wronged”. Because of the irreversibility of the death penalty, it is critically important to prevent wrongful convictions which may result in executions. In reality, although safeguards have been adopted by various criminal justice systems, wrongful convictions still occur from time to time. Take the United States for an example: although the United States Supreme Court has established a stringent judicial review system for capital punishment, wrongful convictions are still unavoidable. According to statistics of the Death Penalty Information Center, from 1973, a total of 157 defendants have been exonerated. With the adoption of new technology (e.g., DNA testing), this trend of reversing wrongful convictions has been accelerated since the late 1990’s. From 1973 to 1999, there were an average of 3.03 exonerations per year; from 2000 to 2013, this number was increased to 4.29 per year. Defendant who were exonerated spent an average of 11.3 years from the moment of one’s death sentence to one’s final exoneration.

Wrongful convictions often cause a series of problems with negative effects, but most of such problems are overlooked by the public. Firstly, wrongfully convicted defendants lose their personal freedom, and suffer from incarceration until one’s exoneration, and some are even wrongfully executed. Secondly, due to wrongful convictions, the real criminals may not have yet been arrested. Not only did the real criminals escape from deserved punishment, they may also continue to commit more crimes. Besides, wrongful convictions waste tremendous human, material and other social resources. Wrongful convictions also weaken the public’s satisfaction with and trust in the criminal justice system.

Reasons that lead to wrongful convictions are various, but are often closely related to the criminal justice system of a nation. Studies of American wrongful convictions revealed a number of key reasons, including eyewitnesses’ false testimonies (misidentification), (coerced) confession by wrongfully convicted defendants, subjective and obstinate opinions by the police/prosecutor, wrong information provided by informants, wrong forensic examination results, prosecutorial misconduct, and lack of effective criminal defense. Compared with the situation of China, besides all of the above factors, scholars pointed out many others such as use of torture, over-reliance upon one’s confession, illegally extracted evidence, falsification of evidence and concealment (by the police), overlook of exculpatory evidence, rejection of reasonable opinions of defense attorneys, and the traditional ‘presumption of guilt’ in practice.

In recent years, frequent exposure of wrongful conviction cases shows increasing attention given by China’s criminal justice system to wrongful convictions. It also reveals serious drawbacks of China’s system. A white paper titled The Judicial Disclosure of Chinese Courts (2013–2016) published by the Supreme People’s Court revealed that in 2016 alone, courts in the whole country corrected 11 new major wrongful conviction cases (involving 17 people). From 2013 to 2016, courts at all levels announced 3718 defendants ‘not guilty’, accepted a total of 16,889 state compensation cases, and compensated a total of 699 million RMB. Many infamous unjust verdicts and wrongful convictions have become classic textbook cases. For example, in 1996 Huugjilt was convicted of rape and homicide, sentenced to death and executed (by shooting). Because the case occurred during a “strike-hard” campaign, it took merely 2 months from the commission of the crime to his execution. Only until 2005 when the real criminal was arrested, was Huugjilt recognized as wrongfully convicted. Huugjilt was officially exonerated (posthumously) in 2014 and his family was compensated for more than 2 million RMB. In 2006, in another case, Nian Bin was identified by the police as the primary suspect of a poisoning case in Pingtan, Fujian province. From 2008 to 2014, Nian Bin’s case was tried multiple times by the Intermediate People’s Court of Fujian (the court of the first instance), the High Court of Fujian (the court of the second instance) and the national Supreme People’s Court. He was sentenced to death three times by the first instance court and his death sentence was affirmed and approved once by the High Court of Fujian. In other words, he was very close to facing death four times. Finally due to insufficient evidence, Nian was announced ‘not guilty’ in 2014 and he received more than one million RMB as a result of compensation afterwards.

Essay III: Deterrence and the Death Penalty

The deterrence effect is one of the most important reasons why people support the death penalty. The deterrence effect of punishment is derived from utilitarianism. Utilitarianism rarely considers motivations and means of one’s behavior but the maximization of benefits incurred by one’s behavior. What is good is to increase the maximum benefit; otherwise it is bad. The deterrence function of punishment uses coercive force to increase the suffering of criminals and therefore makes people fear for the punishment and dare not commit a crime. Scholars who support the deterrence of punishment claim that if people know beforehand that the pain from the punishment of a crime outweighs the gain from a crime, their will to commit a crime could be restrained and their crimes deterred and prevented. Generally speaking traditional deterrence theories believe that the effectiveness of deterrence is determined by three factors: swiftness, certainty and severity of the punishment. That is, once crimes occurred, the faster the punishment is rendered, the more certain the punishment is, and the more severe the punishment is, the more effective the deterrence effect is. The deterrence of punishment can be categorized as two kinds: specific deterrence and general deterrence. The former is to deter possible future crimes of a criminal through punishment of that particular criminal, and the latter is to deter crimes from all potential criminals through punishment of one particular criminal. In general the death penalty is considered the most serious in all kinds of punishment. Because the criminal can never commit a crime again after execution, the deterrence of the death penalty emphasizes the effect of general deterrence.

Many scholars point out that it is subject to debate whether the death penalty has its expected deterrence effect. For example, the reason why most people obey the law is not because of the fear of punishment after one commits a crime, but due to their belief in moral and ethical values. In contrast, a small number of vicious people know very well that their crimes will trigger strict punishment; nevertheless they still defy the law, and punishment (include the death penalty) is difficult to carry a deterrence effect on them. Moreover, the deterrence function of punishment assumes that criminals are capable of rational decision-making and are able to foresee the consequences of their crimes and to weigh potential benefits of their crimes with the consequences. In reality, however, not all criminals have such a capability of making rational decisions.

Does the death penalty have a deterrence effect? Empirical researches by Western social scientists showed that the deterrence effect of the death penalty is far less powerful than we think. For example, in the United States, most of condemned capital criminals are murderer, and quite a few of them committed crimes by passion. In such cases, criminals lose their capability of rational decision-making in the moment of crime. To date, there lacks reliable data to prove that the death penalty can prevent or stop murder effectively. In addition, the presumed deterrence effect of the death penalty on other violent and nonviolent crimes are also doubtful. For example, research shows that in the United States, some states that retain the practice of the death penalty have a higher rate of violent crimes than that of the state without the death penalty. Furthermore, scholars argued it is not true that “the more severe the punishment is, the better the deterrence effect is”, as its marginal deterrence effect decreases (when the severity of the punishment increases). In some circumstances, severe punishment (such as the death penalty) could increase the likelihood of unnecessary crimes by criminals (e.g., a criminal kills a rape victim to cover up the crime and destroys evidence and the dead body). In such circumstances, the use of the death penalty may further stimulate, instead of deter, one’s crimes. Besides, scholars who propose to abolish and replace the death penalty also point out that there are no reliable data to prove that the death penalty has a marginal deterrence effect to any other effective alternative penalties (such as life imprisonment without possibility of parole). That is, there is no proof that the death penalty carries more deterrence effect than that of other alternative penalties.

In China, the range of death penalty application is broader and covers not only violent crimes but also nonviolent crimes (such as drug trafficking, corruption and some economic crimes). In view of no existence of reliable data on crime and the death penalty, empirical studies of the deterrence effect of the death penalty in China are nonexistent. In recent years, some scholars point out that although China always applies severe punishment for drug crimes (e.g., the rate of death sentenced drug offenders stays at a high level), drug crimes keep increasing, thus questioning the deterrence effect of China’s death penalty to drug crimes. For the same reason, the presumed deterrence effect of the death penalty to other crimes (such as corruption cases) should be openly discussed and questioned.

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Liang, B., Liu, J. & Lu, H. Variability of death penalty attitude in China: an empirical test of the Marshall hypotheses. Crime Law Soc Change 72, 269–302 (2019). https://doi.org/10.1007/s10611-018-9809-4

Download citation

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s10611-018-9809-4

Navigation