Skip to main content
Log in

Rumination: Practicing Retrieval of Autobiographical Memories

  • Original Article
  • Published:
Cognitive Therapy and Research Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

Background

People who ruminate about negative personal experiences seem to be engaged in practicing retrieval, with the expected consequences of perpetuating those very memories (see Roediger and Butler in Trends Cogn Sci 15:20–27, 2011).

Method

To provide an experimental model of the effect of retrieval practice on subsequent recall of autobiographical memories, we recruited students with low and high scores on the Ruminative Response Scale (Treynor et al. in Cogn Ther Res 27:247–259, 2003), gave them positive and ruminative cues to generate memories, and cued three rounds of practice of half of the memories from each valence.

Results

A week later, final cued recall for all memories showed strong practice effects, with the exception that ruminators benefited very little from the request to practice positive memories.

Conclusion

Recalling personal memories benefits from prior practice in bringing them to mind. However, these results provide insufficient evidence to recommend training recall of positive autobiographical memories to counteract rumination.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Institutional subscriptions

Fig. 1
Fig. 2

Similar content being viewed by others

Notes

  1. There were actually two counterbalancing factors; see the “Materials” section.

  2. Our experience suggests that undergraduate students do not show very stable scores on the RRS. Our practice in all experiments concerning rumination has always been to set aside data from individuals who do not produce a similar score at the end of the (first) experimental session—a score that keeps them within a few points of the original group designation.

  3. There were no significant differences in the percentages of specific memories generated, according to any factor in the design, all ps > .28, all ηp2 < .02; overall M = 85.5, 95% CI [81.3, 89.9]. Out of the recalled memories on the final test, the same pattern obtained, all ps > . 47 all ηp2 < .02; overall M = 76.3, 95% CI [70.3, 82.4]. The apparent reduction likely resulted from the on-line nature of the test (i.e. no experimenter stressing specificity).

  4. The practice effect on positive-memory recall by ruminators was marginally significant, F(1, 28) = 4.09, MSE = 343.94, p = .053, ηp2 = .13.

  5. An additional analysis of emotion ratings that included a factor for measurement point did reveal a significant main effect for session, F(1, 51) = 7.71, MSE = 0.680, p = .008, ηp2 = .13, and a nonsignificant trend for an interaction of valence with session, F(1, 51) = 3.66, MSE = 0.893, p = .061, ηp2 = .07. On average, ratings were 0.21 points lower at delay—0.05 points lower for negative memories and 0.37 points lower for positive memories. Interpretation must take into account the confound that means were calculated across varying numbers of memories, due both to forgetting and to the uncontrolled conditions for the delayed test.

  6. Readers might notice that ruminators and nonruminators recalled unpracticed positive and negative memories equally well. We assume that that this baseline level of recall reflects the probability that the test cues work similarly to their cuing tendency during the generation phase, as well as the similar tendencies to forget the episodic context of having produced them a week earlier.

References

  • Beck, A. T., Steer, R. A., & Brown, G. K. (1996). Manual for the Beck Depression Inventory (2nd ed.). San Antonio, TX: Psychological Corporation.

    Google Scholar 

  • Berntsen, D. (1998). Involuntary autobiographical memories in and outside the laboratory: How different are they from voluntary autobiographical memories? Memory, 6, 113–141.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Bjork, R. A. (1975). Retrieval as a memory modifier: An interpretation of negative recency and related phenomena. In R. Solso (Ed.), Information processing and cognition: The Loyola Symposium (pp. 123–144). Earlbaum: Hillsdale, NJ.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bradley, M. M., & Lang, P. J. (1999). Affective Norms for English Words (ANEW): Instruction manual and affective ratings (Technical Report C-1). Gainesville: Center for Research in Psychophysiology, University of Florida.

    Google Scholar 

  • Cohen, N., Mor, N., & Henik, A. (2015). Linking executive control and emotional response: A training procedure to reduce rumination. Clinical Psychological Science, 3, 15–25.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Daches, S., Mor, N., & Hertel, P. (2019). Training to inhibit negative content affects memory and rumination. Cognitive Therapy and Research. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10608-019-10023-0.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Everaert, J., Bronstein, M. V., Cannon, T. D., & Joormann, J. (2018). Looking throught tinted glasses: Depression and social anxiety are related to both interpretation biases and inflexible negative interpretations. Clinical Psychological Science, 6, 517–528.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Fawcett, J. M., Benoit, R. G., Gagnepain, P., Salman, A., Bartholdy, S., Bradley, C., et al. (2015). The origins of repetitive thought in rumination: Separating cognitive style from deficits in inhibitory control over memory. Journal of Behavior Therapy and Experimental Psychiatry, 47, 1–8.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hertel, P., Maydon, A., Cottle, J., & Vrijsen, J. (2017). Cognitive bias modification: Retrieval practice to simulate and oppose ruminative memory biases. Clinical Psychological Science, 5, 122–130.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hertel, P. T., Maydon, A., Ogilvie, A., & Mor, N. (2018). Ruminators (unlike others) fail to show suppression-induced forgetting on indirect measures of memory. Clinical Psychological Science, 6, 872–881.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hertel, P., Mor, N., Ferrari, C., Hunt, O., & Agrawal, N. (2014). Looking on the dark side rumination and cognitive-bias modification. Clinical Psychological Science, 2, 714–726.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • James, W. (1890). Habit (p. 24). New York: H. Holt.

    Google Scholar 

  • Joormann, J., & Gotlib, I. H. (2008). Updating the contents of working memory in depression: Interference from irrelevant negative material. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 117, 182–192.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Joormann, J., Siemer, M., & Gotlib, I. H. (2007). Mood regulation in depression: Differential effects of distraction and recall of happy memories on sad mood. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 116, 484–490.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Joormann, J., & Tran, T. B. (2009). Rumination and intentional forgetting of emotional material. Cognition and Emotion., 23, 1233–1246.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Koster, E. H. W., De Lissnyder, E., & De Raedt, R. (2013). Rumination is characterized by valence-specific impairments in switching of attention. Acta Psychologica, 144, 563–570.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lyubomirsky, S., Caldwell, N. D., & Nolen-Hoeksema, S. (1998). Effects of ruminative and distracting responses to depressed mood on retrieval of autobiographical memories. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 75, 166–177.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Mather, M. (2006). Why memories may become more positive as people age. In B. Uttl, N. Ohta, & A. L. Siegenthaler (Eds.), Memory and emotion: Interdisciplinary perspectives (pp. 135–158). Malden: Blackwell Publishing.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Morris, C. D., Bransford, J. D., & Franks, J. J. (1977). Levels of processing versus transfer appropriate processing. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 16, 519–533.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Nolen-Hoeksema, S., Wisco, B. E., & Lyubomirsky, S. (2008). Rethinking rumination. Perspectives on Psychological Science, 3, 400–424.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Roediger, H. L., III, & Butler, A. C. (2011). The critical role of retrieval practice in long-term retention. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 15, 20–27.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Rottenberg, J., Hildner, J. C., & Gotlib, I. H. (2006). Idiographic autobiographical memories in major depressive disorder. Cognition and Emotion, 20, 114–128.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Schlagman, S., & Kvavilashvili, L. (2008). Involuntary autobiographical memories in and outside the laboratory: How different are they from voluntary autobiographical memories? Memory & Cognition, 36, 920–932.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Takano, K., Van Grieken, J., & Raes, F. (2019). Difficulty in updating positive beliefs about negative cognition is associated with increased depressed mood. Journal of Behavior Therapy and Experimental Psychiatry, 64, 22–30.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Treynor, W., Gonzalez, R., & Nolen-Hoeksema, S. (2003). Rumination reconsidered: A psychometric analysis. Cognitive Therapy and Research, 27, 247–259.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Vrijsen, J. N., Dainer-Best, J., Witcraft, S. M., Papini, S., Hertel, P., Beevers, C. P., et al. (2019). Effect of cognitive bias modification-memory on depressive symptoms and autobiographical memory bias: Two independent studies in high-ruminating and dysphoric samples. Cognition and Emotion, 33, 288–304.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Vrijsen, J. N., Hertel, P. T., & Becker, E. S. (2016). Practicing emotionally biased retrieval affects mood and establishes biased recall a week later. Cognitive Therapy and Research, 40, 764–773.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Watkins, E. R., & Nolen-Hoeksema, S. (2014). A habit-goal framework of depressive rumination. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 123, 24–34.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Watson, L. A., Berntsen, D., Kuyken, W., & Watson, E. R. (2012). The characteristics of involuntary and voluntary autobiographical memories in depressed and never depressed individuals. Consciousness and Cognition, 21, 1382–1392.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Young, K. D., Erickson, K., & Drevets, W. C. (2012). Match between cue and memory valence during autobiographical memory recall in depression. Psychological Reports: Mental & Physical Health, 111, 129–148.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Zetsche, U., Burkner, P.-C., & Schulze, L. (2018). Shedding light on the association between repetitive negative thinking and deficits in cognitive control—A meta-analysis. Clinical Psychology Review, 63, 56–65.

    Article  Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgements

The authors are grateful to many students in the lab: Amaris Maydon for help with materials, Sarah Gorban, Victoria Marshall, Sierra Rodriguez, and Mariale Torres for scoring, Etienne Ramirez-Gonzales for conducting some sessions, and Samantha Wilkerson for the collection of pilot data.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Contributions

PTH and PH designed the experiment and constructed materials. Data collection and scoring were performed by PH and PS, who also wrote previous versions of the paper. PTH analyzed the data and wrote the final version, and all authors approved it.

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Paula T. Hertel.

Ethics declarations

Conflict of interest

Paula T. Hertel, Paola Herrera and Pallavi Shamapant declare no conflicts of interest in the preparation of this research, data collection, or publication.

Human and Animal Rights and Informed Consent

This research was approved by the Trinity University Institutional Review Board, and students provided informed consent before participating. Anonymity of responses continue to be preserved.

Additional information

Publisher's Note

Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Hertel, P.T., Herrera, P. & Shamapant, P. Rumination: Practicing Retrieval of Autobiographical Memories. Cogn Ther Res 45, 858–868 (2021). https://doi.org/10.1007/s10608-020-10118-z

Download citation

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s10608-020-10118-z

Keywords

Navigation