Skip to main content

Advertisement

Log in

Implementation Intentions Facilitate Response Inhibition in Children with ADHD

  • Original Article
  • Published:
Cognitive Therapy and Research Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

Attention Deficit/Hyperactivity Disorder (ADHD) is associated with action control problems such as failure to inhibit inappropriate responses. Two studies investigated whether self-regulation by implementation intentions (if-then plans; Gollwitzer, P. M. (1999). Implementation intentions: Strong effects of simple plans. American Psychologist, 54, 493–503) facilitates response inhibition in children with ADHD. In Study 1, children with ADHD who furnished a suppression goal with implementation intentions improved inhibition of an unwanted response on a Go/No-Go task to the same level observed in children without ADHD. Study 2 showed that a combination of implementation intentions and psychostimulant medication resulted in the highest level of suppression performance in children with ADHD. Theoretical and applied implications of these results are discussed.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Fig. 1

Similar content being viewed by others

Notes

  1. For ease of exposition in this paper, the term ADHD is used constantly as an abbreviation for Attention-Deficit/Hyperactivity Disorder according to the DSM-IV subtypes of ADHD (Predominantly Inattentive Type, Combined Type, Predominantly Hyperactive-Impulsive Type) and the ICD-10 category of Hyperkinetic Disorder.

  2. No titration algorithm was used.

References

  • Aarts, H., & Dijksterhuis, A. (2000). Habits as knowledge structures: Automaticity in goal-directed behavior. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 78, 53–63

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Arbeitsgruppe Deutsche Child Behavior Checklist. (1998). Elternfragebogen über das Verhalten von Kindern und Jugendlichen. Deutsche Bearbeitung der Child Behavior Checklist (CBCL/4-18). Köln: Arbeitsgruppe Kinder-, Jugend- und Familiendiagnostik (KJFD). [German consensus version of the Child Behavior Checklist (CBCL/4-18)].

    Google Scholar 

  • Arnsten, A. F. T., & Dudley, A. G. (2005). Methylphenidate improves prefrontal cortical cognitive function through α2 adrenoceptor and dopamine D1 receptor actions: Relevance to therapeutic effects in Attention Deficit Hyperactivity Disorder. Behavioral Brain Functions, 1, 1–9.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Barkley, R. A. (1990). Attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder: A handbook for diagnosis and treatment. New York: Guilford.

    Google Scholar 

  • Barkley, R. A. (1997). ADHD and the nature of self-control. New York: Guilford Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bedard, A., Ickowicz, A., Logan, G. D., Hogg-Johnson, S., Schachar, R., & Tannock, R. (2003). Selective inhibition in children with attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder off and on stimulant medication. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 31, 315–327.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Berlin, L., Bohlin, G., & Rydell, A. (2003). Relations between inhibition, executive functioning, and ADHD symptoms: A longitudinal study from age 5 to 8½ years. Child Neuropsychology, 9, 255–266.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Booth, J. R., Burman, D. D., Meyer, J. R., Lei, Z., Trommer, B. L., Davenport, N. D., Li, W., Parrish, T. B., Gitelman, D. R., & Mesulam, M. M. (2005). Larger deficits in brain networks for response inhibition than for visual selective attention in attention deficit hyperactivity disorder. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 46, 94–11.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Brandstaetter, V., Lengfelder, A., & Gollwitzer, P. M. (2001). Implementation intentions and efficient action initiation. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 81, 946–960.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Clarke, C., Prior, M., & Kinsella, G. (2000). Do executive function deficits differentiate between adolescents with ADHD and Oppositional Defiant/Conduct Disorder? A neuropsychological study using the Six elements test and Hayling sentence completion test. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 28, 403–414.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Cohen, A., Bayer, U. C., Jaudas, A., & Gollwitzer, P. M. (in press). Implementation intentions and executive control: Task switch performance and the Simon effect. Psychological Research.

  • Durston, S. (2003). A review of the biological bases of ADHD: What have we learned from imaging studies? Mental Retardation & Developmental Disabilities Research Reviews, 9, 184–195.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Fischer, M., Barkley, R. A., Smallish, L., & Fletcher, K. (2005). Executive functioning in hyperactive children as young adults: Attention, inhibition, response perseveration, and the impact of comorbidity. Developmental Neuropsychology, 27, 107–133.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Frazier, T. W., Demaree, H. A., & Youngstrom, E. A. (2004). Meta-analysis of intellectual and neuropsychological test performance in attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder. Neuropsychology, 18, 543–555.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Gollwitzer, P. M. (1993). Goal achievement: The role of intentions. In W. Stroebe, & M. Hewstone (Eds.), European review of social psychology (Vol. 4, pp. 141–185). London: Wiley.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gollwitzer, P. M. (1996). The volitional benefits of planning. In P. M. Gollwitzer, & J. A. Bargh (Eds.), The psychology of action: Linking cognition and motivation to behavior (pp. 287–312). New York: Guilford Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gollwitzer, P. M. (1999). Implementation intentions: Strong effects of simple plans. American Psychologist, 54, 493–503.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Gollwitzer, P. M., & Brandstaetter, V. (1997). Implementation intentions and effective goal pursuit. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 73, 186–199.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Gollwitzer, P. M., Bayer, U., & McCulloch, K. (2005). The control of the unwanted. In R. Hassin, J. Uleman, & J. A. Bargh (Eds.), The new unconscious (pp. 485–515). Oxford: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gollwitzer, P. M., & Sheeran, P. (2006). Implementation intentions and goal achievement: A meta-analysis of effects and processes. Advances of Experimental Social Psychology, 38, 69–119.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Groening, M. (Writer). (1990). The call of the Simpsons [Television series episode]. In J. L. Brooks, & S. Simon (Producer), The Simpsons. Los Angeles: Twentieth Century Fox.

  • Halperin, J. M., & Schulz, K. P. (2006). Revisiting the role of the prefrontal cortex in the pathophysiology of ADHD. Psychological Bulletin, 132, 560–581.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Hood, J., Baird, G., Rankin, P. M., & Isaacs, E. (2005) Immediate effects of methylphenidate on cognitive attention skills of children with attention-deficit-hyperactivity disorder. Developmental Medicine and Child Neurology, 47, 408–414.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Jensen, P. S. (2003). Comorbidity and child psychopathology: recommendations for the next decade. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 31, 293–300.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Jensen, P. S., Hinshaw, S. P., Swanson, J. M., Greenhill, L. L., Conners, C. K., Arnold, L. E., Abikoff, H. B., Elliott, G., Hechtman, L., Hoza, B., March, J. S., Newcorn, J. H., Severe, J. B., Vitiello, B., Wells, K., & Wigal, T. (2001). Findings from the NIMH Multimodal Treatment Study of ADHD (MTA): Implications and applications for primary care providers. Journal of Developmental & Behavioral Pediatrics, 22, 60–73.

    CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Lengfelder, A., & Gollwitzer, P. M. (2001). Reflective and reflexive action control in patients with frontal brain lesions. Neuropsychology, 15, 80–100.

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Logan, G. D., & Cowan, W. B. (1984). On the ability to inhibit thought and action: A theory of an act of control. Psychological Review, 91, 295–327.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Manly, T., Anderson, V., Nimmo-Smith, I., Turner, A., Watson, P., & Robertson, I. H. (2001). The differential assessment of children’s attention: The Test of Everyday Attention for Children (TEA-Ch), normative sample and ADHD performance. The Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry and Allied Disciplines, 42, 1065–1081.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Mehta, M. A., Owen, A. M., Sahakian, B. J., Mavaddat, N., Pickard, J. D., & Robbins, T. W. (2000). Methylphenidate enhances working memory by modulating discrete fornatl and parietal lobe regions in the human brain. The Journal of Neuroscience, 20, 1–6.

    Google Scholar 

  • Meichenbaum, D. H., & Goodman, J. (1971). Training impulsive children to talk to themselves: A means of developing self-control. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 77, 115–126.

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Midha, K. K., McKay, G., Rawson, M. J., Korchinski, E. D., & Hubbard, J. W. (2001). Effects of food on the pharmacokinetics of Methylphenidate. Pharmaceutical Research, 18, 1185–1189.

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Miyake, A., Friedman, N. P., Emerson, M. J., Witzki, A. H., & Howerter, A. (2000). The unity and diversity of executive functions and their contributions to complex “frontal lobe” tasks: A latent variable analysis. Cognitive Psychology, 41, 49–100.

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Miyake, A., Friedman, N. P., Rettinger, D. A., Shah, P., & Hegarty, M. (2001). How are visuospatial working memory, executive functioning, and spatial abilities related? A latent-variable analysis. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 130, 621–640.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Nichols, S. L., & Waschbusch, D. A. (2004). A review of the validity of laboratory cognitive tasks used to asses symptoms of ADHD. Child Psychiatry & Human Development, 34, 297–315.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Oosterlaan, J., Logan, G. D., & Sergeant, J. A. (1998). Response inhibition in ADHD, CD, comorbid ADHD + CD, anxious and normal children: a meta-analysis of studies with the stop task. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 39, 411–425.

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Oosterlaan, J., & Sergeant, J. A. (1996). Inhibition in ADHD, aggressive, and anxious children: A biologically based model of child psychopathology. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 24, 19–36.

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Overtoom, C. C., Kenemans, J. L., Verbaten, M. N., Kemner, C., van der Molen, M. W., van Engeland, H., Buitelaar, J. K., & Koelega, H. S. (2002). Inhibition in children with attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder: A psychophysiological study of the stop task. Biological Psychiatry, 51, 668–676.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Orbell, S., Hodgkins, S., & Sheeran, P. (1997). Implementation intentions and the theory of planned behavior. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 23, 945–954.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Owens, J. S., & Hoza, B. (2003). The role of inattention and hyperactivity/impulsivity in the positive illusory bias. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 71, 680–691.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Parks-Stamm, E., Gollwitzer, P. M., & Oettingen, G. (2007). Action control by implementation intentions: Effective cue detection and efficient response initiation. Social Cognition, 25, 248–266.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Pennington, B. F., Bennetto, L., McAleer, O., & Roberts, R. J. (1996). Executive functions and working memory: Theoretical and measurement issues. In G. R. Lyon, & N. A. Krasnegor (Eds.), Attention, memory, and executive function (pp. 327–348). Baltimore, MD, US: Paul H. Brookes Publishing Co.

    Google Scholar 

  • Pliszka, S. R., Glahn, D. C., Semrud-Clikeman, M., Franklin, C., Perez, R., Xiong, J., & Liotti, M. (2006). Neuroimaging of inhibitory control areas in children with ADHD who were treatment naive or in long-term treatment. American Journal of Psychiatry, 163, 1052–1060.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Pliszka, S. R., Liotti, M., & Woldorff, M. G. (2000). Inhibitory control in children with attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder: event-related potential identify the processing component and timing of an impaired right-frontal response-inhibition mechanism. Biological Psychiatry, 48, 238–246.

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Pliszka, S. R., Borcherding, S. H., Spratley, K., Leon, S., & Irick S. (1997). Measuring inhibitory control in children. Developmental and Behavioral Pediatrics, 18, 254–259.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Posner, M. L, & Rothbart, M. K. (2000). Developing mechanisms of self-regulation. Development & Psychpathology, 12, 427–441.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Reid, R., Trout, A. L., & Schartz, M. (2005). Self-regulation interventions for children with ADHD. Exceptional Children, 71, 361–377.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rosvold, H. E., Mirsky, A. F., Sarason, I., Bransome, E. D., & Beck, L. H. (1956). A continuous performance test of brain damage. Journal of Consulting Psychology, 20, 343–350.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Scheres, A., Oosterlaan, J., Geurts, H., Morein-Zamir, S., Meiran, N., Schut, H., Vlasveld, L., & Sergeant, J. A. (2004). Executive functioning in boys with ADHD: Primarily an inhibition deficit? Archives of Clinical Neuropsychology, 19, 569–594.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Seijts, G. H., & Latham, G. P. (2000). The construct of goal-commitment: Measurement and relationships with task performance. In R. Goffin, & E. Helmes (Eds.), Problems and solutions in human assessment (pp. 315–332). Dordrecht: Kluwer Academic.

    Google Scholar 

  • Sheeran, P., & Orbell, S. (1999). Implementation intentions and repeated behavior: Augmenting the predictive validity of the theory of planned behavior. European Journal of Social Psychology, 29, 349–369.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Sheeran, P., Webb, T. L., & Gollwitzer, P. M. (2005). The interplay between goal intentions and implementation intentions. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 31, 87–98.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Smith, A., & Taylor, E. (2006). Response inhibition and hyperactivity in clinical, non-clinical populations: A meta-analysis using the stop task. In R. D. Oades (Ed.), AD/HD and HKS: Current ideas and ways forward (pp. 203–225). Hauppauge: Nova Science Publishers.

    Google Scholar 

  • Smith, A., Taylor, E., Brammer, M., Toone, B., & Rubia, K. (2006). Task-specific hypoactivation in prefrontal and temporoparietal brain regions during motor inhibition and task switching in medication-naïve children and adolescents with ADHD. American Journal of Psychiatry, 163, 1044–1051.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Sonuga-Barke, E. J. S. (2002). Psychological heterogeneity in AD/HD-a dual pathway model of behavior and cognition. Behavioral Brain Research, 130, 29–36.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Tannock, R. (1998). Attention deficit hyperactivity disorder: advances in cognitive, neurobiological, and genetic research. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 39, 65–99.

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • The World Medical Association (2004). World Medical Association declaration of Helsinki: Ethical principles for medical research involving human subjects. Retrieved June 3, 2006 from http://www.wma.net/e/policy/b3.htm.

  • Webb, T. L., & Sheeran, P. (2003). Can implementation intentions help to overcome ego-depletion? Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 39, 279–286.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Webb, T. L., & Sheeran, P. (2007). How do implementation intentions promote goal attainment? A test of component processes. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 43, 295–302.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • WHO (1991). Internationale Klassifikation psychischer Stoerungen: ICD 10, Kapitel V. Bern: Huber. [Tenth revision of the International classification of diseases, Chapter V (F): Mental and behavioral disorders].

    Google Scholar 

  • Willcutt, E. G., Pennington, B. F., Olson, R. K., Chhabildas, N., & Hulslander, J. (2005). Neuropsychological analyses of comorbidity between reading disability and attention deficit hyperactivity disorder: In search of the common deficit. Developmental Neuropsychology, 27, 35–78.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Wu, K. K., Anderson, V., & Castiello, U. (2006). ADHD and working memory: A task switching paradigm. Journal of Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology, 28, 1288–1306.

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Zelazo, P. D., Qu, L., & Müller, U. (2005). Hot and cool aspects of executive function: Relations in early development. In W. Schneider, R. Schumann-Hengsteler, & B. Sodian (Eds.), Young children’s cognitive development: Interrelationships among executive functioning, working memory, verbal ability, and theory of mind (pp. 71–93). Mahwah, NJ, US: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, Publishers.

    Google Scholar 

  • Zhu, J. (2004). Locating volition. Consciousness and Cognition, 13, 302–322.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Caterina Gawrilow.

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Cite this article

Gawrilow, C., Gollwitzer, P.M. Implementation Intentions Facilitate Response Inhibition in Children with ADHD. Cogn Ther Res 32, 261–280 (2008). https://doi.org/10.1007/s10608-007-9150-1

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s10608-007-9150-1

Keywords

Navigation