Skip to main content
Log in

Mating dances and the evolution of language: What’s the next step?

  • Published:
Biology & Philosophy Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

The Darwinian protolanguage hypothesis is one of the most popular theories of the evolution of human language. According to this hypothesis, language evolved through a three stage process involving general increases in intelligence, the emergence of grammatical structure as a result of sexual selection on protomusical songs, and finally the attachment of meaning to the components of those songs. The strongest evidence for the second stage of this process has been considered to be birdsong, and as a result researchers have investigated the existence of various forms of grammar in the production and comprehension of songs by birds. Here, we argue that mating dances are another relevant source of sexually-selected complexity that has until now been largely overlooked by proponents of Darwinian protolanguage, focusing especially on the dances of long-tailed manakins. We end by sketching several lines of research that should be pursued to determine the relevance of mating dances to the evolution of language.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Fig. 1

(Reproduced from Fitch and Hauser 2004, 378)

Fig. 2

(Reproduced from Fitch and Hauser 2004, 378)

Fig. 3

(Reproduced from Corballis 2007, 700)

Fig. 4

Illustration reprinted from (O’Neil 2012) with permission from the author, who retains all rights

Fig. 5

(Illustrations by K.C. Lukianchuk, reproduced from Lukianchuk and Doucet 2014, p 733)

Fig. 6

Similar content being viewed by others

Notes

  1. Notably for the present thesis, chimpanzee gestural communication has been analyzed as a form of dance (Donald 1991, 1998; King 2009).

  2. Following practice in the empirical literature, we will often refer to the manakin mating displays as “dances”, keeping in mind that the degree of similarity between these displays and the most elaborate forms of human dance is one of the issues to be empirically and philosophically evaluated in the present paper. There is no ideal terminology with which to begin, as some of the other options (such as “courtship” or “mating” display) may also be deemed unduly anthropomorphic. So long as we do not begin by interpreting “dance” in an “anthropofabulous” manner (Buckner 2013), it is as good a starting point as any.

  3. Of course in actual spoken language, either the “if” or the “then” can be implicit (i.e. “I’ll go to the store if we are out of milk.”).

  4. For an overview of the cognitive and evolutionary significance of such behavioral flexibility, see (Buckner 2015; Mikhalevich et al. 2017).

  5. Despite the vast evolutionary distance between humans and birds, we should entirely foreclose the possibility that neural structures controlling components of FLB or even important precursors to FLN could be homologous. Even though the architecture of the avian brain is radically different from that of mammalian brains—birds lack a neocortex and white matter—some brain areas like the hippocampus (implicated in higher cognitive functions like declarative memory, transitive inference, sequence learning, and spatial cognition) do appear to be homologous, and there may be more subtle functional or architectural similarities. For reviews of such comparisons, see (Güntürkün and Bugnyar 2016; Reiner et al. 2004).

  6. We are grateful to an anonymous reviewer for suggesting these examples.

References

  • Abe K, Watanabe D (2011) Songbirds possess the spontaneous ability to discriminate syntactic rules. Nat Neurosci 14(8):1067–1074. https://doi.org/10.1038/nn.2869

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Airey DC, Castillo-Juarez H, Casella G, Pollak EJ, DeVoogd TJ (2000) Variation in the volume of zebra finch song control nuclei is heritable: developmental and evolutionary implications. Proc R Soc Lond B: Biol Sci 267(1457):2099–2104

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Anderson ML (2010) Neural reuse: a fundamental organizational principle of the brain. Behav Brain Sci 33(4):245–266. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0140525X10000853 (discussion 266–313)

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Armstrong A (1964) Maori Games and Hakas: Instructions, Words, and Actions. AH & AW Reed, Wellington

    Google Scholar 

  • Barrett L (2016) Why brains are not computers, why behaviorism is not satanism, and why dolphins are not aquatic apes. Behav Anal 39(1):9–23. https://doi.org/10.1007/s40614-015-0047-0

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Barske J, Schlinger BA, Wikelski M, Fusani L (2011) Female choice for male motor skills. Proc R Soc Lond B: Biol Sci, rspb20110382. https://doi.org/10.1098/rspb.2011.0382

  • Berwick RC, Chomsky N (2015) Why only us: language and evolution. MIT Press, Cambridge

    Google Scholar 

  • Berwick RC, Chomsky N (2017) Why only us: recent questions and answers. J Neurolinguist 43:166–177. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jneuroling.2016.12.002

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bian X, Chandler T, Laird W, Pinilla A, Peters R (2017) Integrating evolutionary biology with digital arts to quantify ecological constraints on vision-based behaviour. Methods Ecol Evol Early View. https://doi.org/10.1111/2041-210X.12912

    Google Scholar 

  • Bod R (2009) From exemplar to grammar: a probabilistic analogy-based model of language learning. Cogn Sci 33(5):752–793. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1551-6709.2009.01031.x

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Borgia G (1995) Complex male display and female choice in the spotted bowerbird: specialized functions for different bower decorations. Anim Behav 49(5):1291–1301

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bortolotti GR, Blas J, Negro JJ, Tella JL (2006) A complex plumage pattern as an honest social signal. Anim Behav 72(2):423–430

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bowling DL, Fitch WT (2015) Do animal communication systems have phonemes? Trends Cogn Sci 19(10):555–557

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Buckner C (2013) Morgan’s Canon, meet Hume’s Dictum: avoiding anthropofabulation in cross-species comparisons. Biol Philos 28(5):853–871

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Buckner C (2015) A property cluster theory of cognition. Philos Psychol 28(3):307–336

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Clark A (1999) An embodied cognitive science? Trends Cogn Sci 3(9):345–351

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Coccon F, Schlinger BA, Fusani L (2012) Male Golden-collared Manakins Manacus vitellinus do not adapt their courtship display to spatial alteration of their court. Ibis 154(1):173–176

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Corballis MC (2007) Recursion, Language, and Starlings. Cogn Sci 31(4):697–704. https://doi.org/10.1080/15326900701399947

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Darwin C (1871) The descent of man and selection in relation to sex, 1st edn. John Murray, London

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Day LB, Westcott DA, Olster DH (2005) Evolution of bower complexity and cerebellum size in bowerbirds. Brain Behav Evol 66(1):62–72

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Day LB, Fusani L, Kim C, Schlinger BA (2011) Sexually dimorphic neural phenotypes in golden-collared manakins (Manacus vitellinus). Brain Behav Evol 77(3):206–218

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • De Jaegher H, Di Paolo E, Gallagher S (2010) Can social interaction constitute social cognition? Trends Cogn Sci 14(10):441–447

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Donald M (1991) Origins of the modern mind: Three stages in the evolution of culture and cognition. Harvard University Press, Cambridge

    Google Scholar 

  • Donald M (1998) Mimesis and the executive suite: missing links in language evolution. In: Hurford JR, Studdert-Kennedy M, Knight C (eds) Approaches to the evolution of language: social and cognitive bases. Cambridge University Press, Cambridge

    Google Scholar 

  • DuVal EH (2013) Does cooperation increase helpers’ later success as breeders? A test of the skills hypothesis in the cooperatively displaying lance-tailed manakin. J Anim Ecol 82(4):884–893

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Edelman AJ, McDonald DB (2014) Structure of male cooperation networks at long-tailed manakin leks. Anim Behav 97:125–133. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.anbehav.2014.09.004

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Eltz T, Sager A, Lunau K (2005) Juggling with volatiles: exposure of perfumes by displaying male orchid bees. J Comp Physiol A 191(7):575–581

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Feenders G, Liedvogel M, Rivas M, Zapka M, Horita H, Hara E, Jarvis ED (2008) Molecular mapping of movement-associated areas in the avian brain: a motor theory for vocal learning origin. PLoS One 3(3):e1768. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0001768

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Fitch WT (2010) The evolution of language. Cambridge University Press, Cambridge

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Fitch WT (2013) Musical protolanguage: Darwin’s theory of language evolution revisited. In: Bolhuis JJ, Everaert M (eds) Birdsong, speech, and language. Exploring the evolution of mind and brain, The MIT Press, Cambridge, pp 489–503

  • Fitch WT (2014) Toward a computational framework for cognitive biology: unifying approaches from cognitive neuroscience and comparative cognition. Phys Life Rev 11(3):329–364. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.plrev.2014.04.005

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Fitch WT, Hauser MD (2004) Computational constraints on syntactic processing in a nonhuman primate. Science 303(5656):377–380

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Foster MS (1981) Cooperative behavior and social organization of the Swallow-tailed Manakin (Chiroxiphia caudata). Behav Ecol Sociobiol 9(3):167–177

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Franks B (1995) On explanation in the cognitive sciences: competence, idealization, and the failure of the classical cascade. Br J Philos Sci 46(4):475–502

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Fusani L, Giordano M, Day LB, Schlinger BA (2007) High-speed video analysis reveals individual variability in the courtship displays of male golden-collared manakins. Ethology 113(10):964–972

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Gentner TQ, Fenn KM, Margoliash D, Nusbaum HC (2006) Recursive syntactic pattern learning by songbirds. Nature 440(7088):1204–1207. https://doi.org/10.1038/nature04675

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Graham M (1973) The Notebooks of Martha Graham. Harcourt, Eagle Farm

    Google Scholar 

  • Grodzinsky Y (2000) The neurology of syntax: language use without Broca’s area. Behav Brain Sci 23(01):1–21

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Güntürkün O (2005) The avian “prefrontal cortex”and cognition. Curr Opin Neurobiol 15(6):686–693

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Güntürkün O, Bugnyar T (2016) Cognition without cortex. Trends Cogn Sci 20(4):291–303

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hauser MD, Chomsky N, Fitch WT (2002) The faculty of language: What is it, who has it, and how did it evolve? Science 298(5598):1569–1579

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • King BJ (2009) The dynamic dance: nonvocal communication in African great apes. Harvard University Press, Cambridge

    Google Scholar 

  • Lake BM, Salakhutdinov R, Tenenbaum JB (2015) Human-level concept learning through probabilistic program induction. Science 350(6266):1332–1338

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lea SEG, Dittrich WH (2000) What do birds see in moving video images. In: Fagot J (ed) Picture perception in animals. Psychology Press, Hove, pp 143–180

    Google Scholar 

  • Lindsay WR, Houck JT, Giuliano CE, Day LB (2015) Acrobatic courtship display coevolves with brain size in manakins (Pipridae). Brain Behav Evol 85(1):29–36

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lukianchuk KC, Doucet SM (2014) Cooperative courtship display in Long-tailed Manakins Chiroxiphia linearis: predictors of courtship success revealed through full characterization of display. J Ornithol 155(3):729–743

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • McDonald DB (1993) Delayed plumage maturation and orderly queues for status: a manakin mannequin experiment. Ethology 94(1):31–45. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1439-0310.1993.tb00545.x

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • McDonald DB (2010) A spatial dance to the music of time in the leks of long-tailed manakins. In: Macedo R (ed) Advances in the study of behavior, vol 42. Academic Press, Cambridge, pp 55–81. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0065-3454(10)42002-1

    Google Scholar 

  • Mikhalevich I, Powell R, Logan C (2017) Is behavioural flexibility evidence of cognitive complexity? How evolution can inform comparative cognition. Interface Focus 7(3):20160121

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Mooney R (1999) Sensitive periods and circuits for learned birdsong. Curr Opin Neurobiol 9(1):121–127

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Moravec ML, Striedter GF, Burley NT (2010) “Virtual Parrots” confirm mating preferences of female budgerigars. Ethology 116(10):961–971. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1439-0310.2010.01809.x

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Mori M, MacDorman KF, Kageki N (2012) The uncanny valley [from the field]. IEEE Robot Autom Mag 19(2):98–100

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Müller FM (1861) The theoretical stage, and the origin of language. In: Lectures on the science of language. Longman, Green, Longman, and Roberts, London

  • Müller R-A, Basho S (2004) Are nonlinguistic functions in “Broca’s area” prerequisites for language acquisition? fMRI findings from an ontogenetic viewpoint. Brain Lang 89(2):329–336

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Neave N, McCarty K, Freynik J, Caplan N, Hönekopp J, Fink B (2011) Male dance moves that catch a woman’s eye. Biol Let 7(2):221–224. https://doi.org/10.1098/rsbl.2010.0619

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Nowicki S, Searcy WA, Peters S (2002) Brain development, song learning and mate choice in birds: a review and experimental test of the “nutritional stress hypothesis”. J Comp Physiol A: Neuroethol Sensory Neural Behav Physiol 188(11):1003–1014

    Google Scholar 

  • O’Neil D (2012) Evolution of modern humans: archaic human culture. https://www2.palomar.edu/anthro/homo2/mod_homo_3.htm. 14 Nov 2017

  • Olkowicz S, Kocourek M, Lučan RK, Porteš M, Fitch WT, Herculano-Houzel S, Němec P (2016) Birds have primate-like numbers of neurons in the forebrain. Proc Natl Acad Sci 113(26):7255–7260

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ophir AG, Galef BG (2003) Female Japanese quail affiliate with live males that they have seen mate on video. Anim Behav 66(2):369–375

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Patterson-Kane E, Nicol CJ, Foster TM, Temple W (1997) Limited perception of video images by domestic hens. Anim Behav 53(5):951–963

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Penke M, Rosenbach A (2004) What counts as evidence in linguistics?: An introduction. Stud Lang Int J Spons Found “Found Lang” 28(3):480–526. https://doi.org/10.1075/sl.28.3.03pen

    Google Scholar 

  • Price PJ, Ostendorf M, Shattuck-Hufnagel S, Fong C (1991) The use of prosody in syntactic disambiguation. J Acoust Soc Am 90(6):2956–2970

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Prum RO (2012) Aesthetic evolution by mate choice: Darwin’s really dangerous idea. Philos Trans R Soc Lond B: Biol Sci 367(1600):2253–2265

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Pukui MK (1942) The Hula, Hawaii’s Own Dance. Hula: Hist Perspect 30:70–73

    Google Scholar 

  • Reiner A, Perkel DJ, Bruce LL, Butler AB, Csillag A, Kuenzel W, Jarvis ED (2004) Revised nomenclature for avian telencephalon and some related brainstem nuclei. J Comp Neurol 473(3):377–414

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Scherer U, Godin J-GJ, Schuett W (2017) Validation of 2D-animated pictures as an investigative tool in the behavioural sciences: a case study with a West African cichlid fish, Pelvicachromis pulcher. Ethology 123(8):560–570. https://doi.org/10.1111/eth.12630

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Scott-Phillips TC (2015) Nonhuman primate communication, pragmatics, and the origins of language. Curr Anthropol 56(1):56–80

  • Selting M (1996) On the interplay of syntax and prosody in the constitution of turn-constructional units and turns in conversation. Q Publ Int Pragmat Assoc (IPrA) 6(3):371–388

    Google Scholar 

  • Starr F, Cunningham M (1968) Changes: notes on choreography. Something Else Press, New York

    Google Scholar 

  • Stein AC, Uy JAC (2006) Plumage brightness predicts male mating success in the lekking golden-collared manakin, Manacus vitellinus. Behav Ecol 17(1):41–47

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Stevens C, McKechnie S (2005) Thinking in action: thought made visible in contemporary dance. Cogn Process 6(4):243–252

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Taylor AH, Hunt GR, Holzhaider JC, Gray RD (2007) Spontaneous metatool use by New Caledonian crows. Curr Biol 17(17):1504–1507

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Trainer JM, McDonald DB, Learn WA (2002) The development of coordinated singing in cooperatively displaying long-tailed manakins. Behav Ecol 13(1):65–69. https://doi.org/10.1093/beheco/13.1.65

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Vicari G, Adenzato M (2014) Is recursion language-specific? Evidence of recursive mechanisms in the structure of intentional action. Conscious Cogn 26:169–188

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • von Humboldt W (1836) Über die Kawi-Sprache auf der Insel Java. Druckerei der Königlichen Akademie der Wissenschaften, Berlin

    Google Scholar 

  • Ward K-A (2012) Female mate-searching strategies and behavioural correlates of copulation success in lekking Long-tailed Manakins (Chiroxiphia linearis) (MSc). University of Windsor. http://scholar.uwindsor.ca/etd/301/

  • Watanabe S (2006) The neural basis of cognitive flexibility in birds. In: Wasserman EA, Zentall TR (eds) Comparative cognition: experimental explorations of animal intelligence. Oxford University Press, Oxford, pp 619–639

    Google Scholar 

  • Woo KL, Rieucau G (2011) From dummies to animations: a review of computer-animated stimuli used in animal behavior studies. Behav Ecol Sociobiol 65(9):1671

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Zednik C, Jäkel F (2016) Bayesian reverse-engineering considered as a research strategy for cognitive science. Synthese 193(12):3951–3985

    Article  Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgements

The authors would like to thank Bruce Rushing, Bryce Huebner, Carrie Figdor, Jo Anne Fleischhauer, Lainy Day, Lauren Alpert, Nathan Gabriel, Simon Brown, Susan Balenger, and two anonymous reviewers for discussions and comments on earlier versions of this paper.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Cameron Buckner.

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Buckner, C., Yang, K. Mating dances and the evolution of language: What’s the next step?. Biol Philos 32, 1289–1316 (2017). https://doi.org/10.1007/s10539-017-9605-z

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s10539-017-9605-z

Keywords

Navigation