Skip to main content
Log in

Effects of neurofeedback and working memory-combined training on executive functions in healthy young adults

  • Original Article
  • Published:
Psychological Research Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

Given the interest in improving executive functions, the present study examines a promising combination of two training techniques: neurofeedback training (NFT) and working memory training (WMT). NFT targeted increasing the amplitude of individual’s upper Alpha frequency band at the parietal midline scalp location (Pz), and WMT consisted of an established computerized protocol with working memory updating and set-shifting components. Healthy participants (n = 140) were randomly allocated to five combinations of training, including visual search training used as an active control training for the WMT; all five groups were compared to a sixth silent control group receiving no training. All groups were evaluated before and after training for resting-state electroencephalogram (EEG) and behavioral executive function measures. The participants in the silent control group were unaware of this procedure, and received one of the training protocols only after study has ended. Results demonstrated significant improvement in the practice tasks in all training groups including non-specific influence of NFT on resting-state EEG spectral topography. There was only a near transfer effect (improvement in working memory task) for WMT, which remained significant in the delayed post-test (after 1 month), in comparison to silent control group but not in comparison to active control training group. The NFT + WMT combined group showed improved mental rotation ability both in the post-training and in the follow-up evaluations. This improvement, however, did not differ significantly from that in the silent control group. We conclude that the current training protocols, including their combination, have very limited influence on the executive functions that were assessed in this study.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Subscribe and save

Springer+ Basic
$34.99 /Month
  • Get 10 units per month
  • Download Article/Chapter or eBook
  • 1 Unit = 1 Article or 1 Chapter
  • Cancel anytime
Subscribe now

Buy Now

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Fig. 1
Fig. 2
Fig. 3
Fig. 4
Fig. 5
Fig. 6
Fig. 7
Fig. 8
Fig. 9
Fig. 10
Fig. 11
Fig. 12
Fig. 13

Similar content being viewed by others

Notes

  1. Improvement in the training task leads to improvement on tasks that are similar to the one participants were trained on.

  2. Improvement in the training task can be generalized to improvement on tasks that are very different from the one participants were trained on, but still require similar cognitive processes.

  3. Handedness was determined based on the preferred hand for writing, assessed on a three-point scale (1 = right, 2 = left, 3 = either hand/ambidextrous).

  4. The Intelligence Rating Score is a well-established tool for intelligence assessment comprised from four sub-tests presented in a multiple-choice format: (1) the Otis-R, which measures the ability to understand and carry out verbal instructions; (2) Similarities-R, assesses verbal abstraction and categorization; (3) Arithmetic-R, which measures mathematical reasoning, concentration, and concept manipulation; and (4) Raven’s Progressive Matrices-R, that measures non-verbal abstract reasoning and visual-spatial problem-solving abilities. The sum of the scores for the four tests forms a validated measure of general intelligence, scored on a 9-point scale, scaled between 10 and 90, with a 10-point increment at each score (Gal 1986). The correlation between the general intelligence rating score and the WAIS total IQ is > 0.90 (Kaplan et al. 2002).

  5. In general, the test re-test reliability of quantitative EEG is an exponential function of sample length in which 20 s epochs are approximately 0.8 reliable, 40 s approximately 0.9 reliable and 60 s asymptotes at approximately 0.95 reliability (Burgess and Gruzelier 1993; Van Albada and Robinson 2007).

  6. It is important to mention here that this brief executive functions battery provided a relatively reasonable coverage of the executive function domain. However, this coverage was nonetheless incomplete. For example, instead of measuring working memory updating, we indirectly measured other working memory functions. One of them is conceptually related to capacity (“Alternative Cost”). The other (“Tau”) is described in the main text.

  7. In the practice phase, all responses were followed by a feedback (correct/error), see Fig. 2.

  8. The validation of Tau as an index of working memory functioning comes from three sources. One is the correlational studies (e.g., Schmiedeck et al. 2007), showing a high correlation between individual differences in Tau and in working memory. The second is the fact that Tau (and not Sigma or Mu) is strongly influenced by the working memory demands of the task (mapping arbitrariness, especially when the number of alternatives is high, Shahar et al. 2014, see also, 2018). Lastly, mathematical modeling reported in these papers has linked Tau to the rate of retrieval from working memory, with this model being superior to alternative models such as those linking Tau mainly to attentional lapses or to the rate of evidence accumulation.

  9. We considered the first 4 trials of the mental rotation test as practice trials and, therefore, analysis was conducted without them, on 60 trials per participant.

  10. The proportion of discarded trials is the sum of all three evaluations (pre, post, follow-up).

  11. Since two participants from the combined training (NFT + WMT) had extreme NFT results, we calculate the interaction without them and the results remained similar [F (9, 324) = 0.98, p = .461, η2p = .02, BF10 = 0.06, indicating strong support for H0].

  12. Similar results were found after excluding the two participants from the combined training (NFT + active control training group) who had extreme NFT results [F (9, 324) = 0.51, p = .864, η2p = .01, BF10 = 0.02, indicating strong support for H0].

References

  • Alfonso, J. P., Caracuel, A., Delgado-Pastor, L. C., & Verdejo-García, A. (2011). Combined goal management training and mindfulness meditation improve executive functions and decision-making performance in abstinent polysubstance abusers. Drug and alcohol dependence, 117, 78–81.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Altmann, E. M., & Gray, W. D. (2008). An integrated model of cognitive control in task switching. Psychological Review, 115, 602–639.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Arbiv, D. C., & Meiran, N. (2015). Performance on the antisaccade task predicts dropout from cognitive training. Intelligence, 49, 25–31.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Association, W. M. (2013). World Medical Association Declaration of Helsinki: ethical principles for medical research involving human subjects. JAMA, 310, 2191–2194.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Au, J., Karsten, C., Buschkuehl, M., & Jaeggi, S. M. (2017). Optimizing transcranial direct current stimulation protocols to promote long-term learning. Journal of Cognitive Enhancement, 1, 65–72.‏.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Au, J., Sheehan, E., Tsai, N., Duncan, G. J., Buschkuehl, M., & Jaeggi, S. M. (2015). Improving fluid intelligence with training on working memory: a meta-analysis. Psychonomic bulletin & review, 22, 366–377.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Basar, E. (2006). The theory of the whole-brain-work. International Journal of Psychophysiology, 60, 133–138.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Burgess, A., & Gruzelier, J. (1993). Individual reliability of amplitude distribution in topographical mapping of EEG. Electroencephalography and clinical Neurophysiology, 86, 219–223.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Buschkuehl, M., Jaeggi, S. M., & Jonides, J. (2012). Neuronal effects following working memory training. Developmental Cognitive Neuroscience, 2, 167–179.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Cooper, N. R., Croft, R. J., Dominey, S. J., Burgess, A., P., & Gruzelier, J., H (2003). Paradox lost? Exploring the role of alpha oscillations during externally vs. internally directed attention and the implications for idling and inhibition hypotheses. International Journal of Psychophysiology, 47, 65–74.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Diamond, A., Barnett, W. S., Thomas, J., & Munro, S. (2007). Preschool program improves cognitive control. Science, 318, 1387–1388.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Ditye, T., Jacobson, L., Walsh, V., & Lavidor, M. (2012). Modulating behavioral inhibition by tDCS combined with cognitive training. Experimental Brain Research, 219, 363–369.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Egner, T., Zech, T. F., & Gruzelier, J. H. (2004). The effects of neurofeedback training on the spectral topography of the electroencephalogram. Clinical Neurophysiology, 115(11), 2452–2460.‏.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Enriquez-Geppert, S., Huster, R. J., & Herrmann, C. S. (2017). EEG-neurofeedback as a tool to modulate cognition and behavior: a review tutorial. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, 11, 51–58.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Escolano, C., Aguilar, M., & Minguez, J. (2011). EEG-based upper alpha neurofeedback training improves working memory performance. In Engineering in medicine and biology society, EMBC, 2011 Annual International Conference of the IEEE, pp. 2327–2330.

  • Finnigan, S., & Robertson, I. H. (2011). Resting EEG theta power correlates with cognitive performance in healthy older adults. Psychophysiology, 48, 1083–1087.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Fox, K. C., Dixon, M. L., Nijeboer, S., Girn, M., Floman, J. L., Lifshitz, M., … Christoff, K. (2016). Functional neuroanatomy of meditation: A review and meta-analysis of 78 functional neuroimaging investigations. Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews, 65, 208–228.&#8207.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Friedman, N. P., & Miyake, A. (2017). Unity and diversity of executive functions: Individual differences as a window on cognitive structure. Cortex, 86, ‏‏186–204.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Gal, R. (1986). A portrait of the Israeli soldier. Westport‏: Greenwood Publishing Group.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gevins, A., & Smith, M. E. (2000). Neurophysiological measures of working memory and individual differences in cognitive ability and cognitive style. Cerebral Cortex, 10, 829–839.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Gevins, A., Zeitlin, G. M., Yingling, C. D., Doyle, J. C., Dedon, M. F., Schaffer, R. E., Yeager, C. L. (1979). EEG patterns during ‘cognitive’ tasks. I. Methodology and analysis of complex behaviors. Electroencephalography and Clinical Neurophysiology, 47, 693–703.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Gevins, A. S., Zeitlin, G. M., Doyle, J. C., Schaffer, R. E., & Callaway, E. (1979). EEG patterns during ‘cognitive’ tasks. II. Analysis of controlled tasks. Electroencephalography and Clinical Neurophysiology, 47, 704–710.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Gevins, A. S., Zeitlin, G. M., Doyle, J. C., Yingling, C. D., Schaffer, R. E., Callaway, E., & Yeager, C. L. (1979). Electroencephalogram correlates of higher cortical functions. Science, 203, 665–668.&#8207.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Gordon, S., Todder, D., Deutsch, I., Garbi, D., Getter, N., & Meiran, N. (2018). Are resting state spectral power measures related to executive functions in healthy young adults? Neuropsychologia, 108, 61–72.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Goyal, M., Singh, S., Sibinga, E. M., Gould, N. F., Rowland-Seymour, A., Sharma, R., Ranasinghe, P. D. (2014). Meditation programs for psychological stress and well-being: a systematic review and meta-analysis. JAMA Internal Medicine, 174, 357–368.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Gruzelier, J. H. (2014a). EEG-neurofeedback for optimizing performance. I: a review of cognitive and affective outcome in healthy participants. Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews, 44, 124–141.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Gruzelier, J. (2014b). EEG-neurofeedback for optimizing performance. III: a review of methodological and theoretical considerations. Neuroscience Biobehavioral Reviews, 44, 159–182.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Guez, J., Rogel, A., Getter, N., Keha, E., Cohen, T., Amor, T., & Todder, D. (2015). Influence of electroencephalography neurofeedback training on episodic memory: A randomized, sham-controlled, double-blind study. Memory, 23, 683–694.&#8207.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Hanslmayr, S., Sauseng, P., Doppelmayr, M., Schabus, M., & Klimesch, W. (2005). Increasing individual upper alpha power by neurofeedback improves cognitive performance in human subjects. Journal of Applied Psychophysiology and Biofeedback, 30, 1–10.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Harrison, T. L., Shipstead, Z., Hicks, K. L., Hambrick, D. Z., Redick, T. S., & Engle, R. W. (2013). Working memory training may increase working memory capacity but not fluid intelligence. Psychological Science, 24, 2409–2419.&#8207.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Hosseini, S. H., Pritchard-Berman, M., Sosa, N., Ceja, A., & Kesler, S. R. (2016). Task-based neurofeedback training: A novel approach toward training executive functions. NeuroImage, 134, 153–159.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Hummel, F., Andres, F., Altenmüller, E., Dichgans, J., & Gerloff, C. (2002). Inhibitory control of acquired motor programs in the human brain. Brain, 125, 404–420.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Hyun, J. S., & Luck, S. J. (2007). Visual working memory as the substrate for mental rotation. Psychonomic Bulletin and Review, 14, 154–158.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Jaeggi, S. M., Buschkuehl, M., Jonides, J., & Perrig, W. J. (2008). Improving fluid intelligence with training on working memory. In Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 105, pp. 6829–6833.

  • JASP Team. (2018). JASP (Version 0.8.6) [Computer software].

  • Johnstone, S. J., Roodenrys, S. J., Johnson, K., Bonfield, R., & Bennett, S. J. (2017). Game-based combined cognitive and neurofeedback training using Focus Pocus reduces symptom severity in children with diagnosed AD/HD and subclinical AD/HD. International Journal of Psychophysiology, 116, 32–44.&#8207.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Just, M. A., & Carpenter, P. A. (1985). Cognitive coordinate systems: accounts of mental rotation and individual differences in spatial ability. Psychological Review, 92, 137–172.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Kaiser, D. A., & Sterman, M. B. (2001). Automatic artifact detection, overlapping windows, and state transitions. Journal of Neurotherapy, 4, 85–92.&#8207.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kaplan, Z., Weiser, M., Reichenberg, A., Rabinowitz, J., Caspi, A., Bodner, E., & Zohar, J. (2002). Motivation to serve in the military influences vulnerability to future posttraumatic stress disorder. Psychiatry Research, 109, 45–49.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • ‏Karbach, J., & Verhaeghen, P. (2014). Making working memory work: A meta-analysis of executive-control and working memory training in older adults. Psychological Science, 25, 2027–2037.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Kashdan, T. B., & Rottenberg, J. (2010). Psychological flexibility as a fundamental aspect of health. Clinical Psychology Review, 30, 865–878.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Klimesch, W. (1999). EEG alpha and theta oscillations reflect cognitive and memory performance: a review and analysis. Brain Research Reviews, 29, 169–195.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Klimesch, W., Doppelmayr, M., Schwaiger, J., Auinger, P., & Winkler, T. (1999). Paradoxical’alpha synchronization in a memory task. Cognitive Brain Research, 7, 493–501.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Koch, I., Gade, M., Schuch, S., & Philipp, A. M. (2009). The role of inhibition in task switching: A review. Psychonomic Bulletin and Review, 17, 1–14.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Laming, D. (1979). Choice reaction performance following an error. Acta Psychologica, 43, 199–224.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Langer, N., von Bastian, C. C., Wirz, H., Oberauer, K., & Jäncke, L. (2013). The effects of working memory training on functional brain network efficiency. Cortex, 49, 2424–2438.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Logemann, H. A., Lansbergen, M. M., Van Os, T. W., Böcker, K. B., & Kenemans, J. L. (2010). The effectiveness of EEG-feedback on attention, impulsivity and EEG: a sham feedback controlled study. Neuroscience Letters, 479, 49–53.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Mayr, U., & Keele, S. W. (2000). Changing internal constraints on action: The role of backward inhibition. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 129, 4–26.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Mayr, U., & Kliegl, R. (2000). Task-set switching and long-term memory retrieval. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 26, 1124–1140.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Mayr, U., & Kliegl, R. (2003). Differential effects of cue changes and task changes on task-set selection costs. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 29, 362–372.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Meiran, N., Chorev, Z., & Sapir, A. (2000). Component processes in task switching. Cognitive Psychology, 41, 211–253.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Meiran, N., Hsieh, S., & Chang, C. (2011). “Smart inhibition”: Electrophysiological evidence for the suppression of conflict-generating task rules during task-switching. Cognitive, Affective, and Behavioral Neuroscience, 11, 299–308.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Meiran, N., Hsieh, S., & Dimov, E. (2010). Resolving task rule incongruence during task switching by competitor rule suppression. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 36, 992–1002.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Melby-Lervåg,M., & Hulme,C. (2016).There is no convincing evidence that working memory training is effective: A reply to Auet al. (2014) and Karbach and Verhaeghen (2014). Psychonomic Bulletin and Review, 23, 324–330.‏

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Mirifar, A., Beckmann, J., & Ehrlenspiel, F. (2017). Neurofeedback as supplementary training for optimizing athletes’ performance: A systematic review with implications for future research. Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews, 75, 419–432.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Miyake, A., Friedman, N. P., Emerson, M. J., Witzki, A. H., Howerter, A., & Wager, T. D. (2000). The unity and diversity of executive functions and their contributions to complex “frontal lobe” tasks: A latent variable analysis. Cognitive Psychology, 41, 49–100.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Nan, W., Rodrigues, J. P., Ma, J., & Qu, X. (2012). Individual alpha neurofeedback training effect on short term memory. Current Psychiatry Reports, 14, 536–542.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Oken, B. S., Flegal, K., Zajdel, D., Kishiyama, S., Haas, M., & Peters, D. (2008). Expectancy effect: impact of pill administration on cognitive performance in healthy seniors. Journal of Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology, 30, 7–17.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Park, S. H., Seo, J. H., Kim, Y. H., & Ko, M. H. (2014). Long-term effects of transcranial direct current stimulation combined with computer-assisted cognitive training in healthy older adults. Neuroreport, 25, 122–126.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Patel, R., Spreng, R. N., & Turner, G. R. (2013). Functional brain changes following cognitive and motor skills training: a quantitative meta-analysis. Neurorehabilitation and neural repair, 27, 187–199.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Rabbitt, P. M. (1966). Errors and error correction in choice-response tasks. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 71, 264–272.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Redick, T. S., Shipstead, Z., Harrison, T. L., Hicks, K. L., Fried, D. E., Hambrick, D. Z., Engle, R. W. (2013). No evidence of intelligence improvement after working memory training: a randomized, placebo-controlled study. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General. 142, 359.‏.

  • Rouder, J. N., Morey, R. D., Speckman, P. L., & Province, J. M. (2012). Default Bayes factors for ANOVA designs. Journal of Mathematical Psychology, 56, 356–374.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Schmiedek, F., Oberauer, K., Wilhelm, O., Süss, H. M., & Wittmann, W. W. (2007). Individual differences in components of reaction time distributions and their relations to working memory and intelligence. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 136, 414–429.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Shahar, N., & Meiran, N. (2015). Learning to control actions: transfer effects following a procedural cognitive control computerized training. PloS One. 10, e0119992‏.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Shahar, N., Pereg, M., Teodorescu, A. R., Moran, R., Karmon-Presser, A., & Meiran, N. (2018). Formation of abstract task representations: Exploring dosage and mechanisms of working memory training effects. Cognition, 181, 151–159.&#8207.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Shahar, N., Teodorescu, A. R., Usher, M., Pereg, M., & Meiran, N. (2014). Selective influence of working memory load on exceptionally slow reaction times. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 143, 1837‏.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Shepard, J., & Metzler (1971). Mental rotation of three-dimensional objects. Science, 701–703.

  • Soffer-Dudek, N., Todder, D., Shelef, L., Deutsch, I., & Gordon, S. (2018). A neural correlate for common trait dissociation: Decreased EEG connectivity is related to dissociative absorption. Journal of Personality.

  • Soveri, A., Antfolk, J., Karlsson, L., Salo, B., & Laine, M. (2017). Working memory training revisited: A multi-level meta-analysis of n-back training studies. Psychonomic Bulletin and Review. 1–20.

  • Van Albada, S. J., & Robinson, P.A. (2007). Transformation of arbitrary distributions to the normal distribution with application to EEG test–retest reliability. Journal of neuroscience methods, 161, 205–211.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Wan, F., Nan, W., Vai, M. I., & Rosa, A. (2014). Resting alpha activity predicts learning ability in alpha neurofeedback. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, 8, 500‏‏‏.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Zoefel, B., Huster, R. J., & Herrmann, C. S. (2011). Neurofeedback training of the upper alpha frequency band in EEG improves cognitive performance. Neuroimage, 54, 1427–1431.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgements

The study was supported by the Israel Defense Forces (IDF) Medical Corps and Directorate of Defense Research & Development, Israeli Ministry of Defense (IMOD DDR&D). We would like to thank Col. Dr. Erez Carmon, Col. Dr. Eyal Fructer and Lut. Idit Oz for their belief in the value of this research and, therefore, supporting this study. Dr. Arik Eisenkraft and Dr. Linn Wagnert-Avraham from the Institute for Research in Military Medicine, the Hebrew University Faculty of Medicine, Jerusalem for their help in purchasing the equipment needed for the study and handling the budget. As well as Mr. Erez Gordon and Prof. Robert Thatcher for advising in regard to analyzing the data.

Funding

The study was supported by the Israel Defense Forces (IDF) Medical Corps and Directorate of Defense Research & Development, Israeli Ministry of Defense (IMOD DDR&D).

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Contributions

SG had full access to all of the data in the study and takes responsibility for the integrity of the data and the accuracy of the data analysis. Study concept and design: SG, DT, and NM. Acquisition of data: SG, ID, and NS. Analysis and interpretation of data: all the authors. Drafting of the manuscript: SG, NM, ID and DT. Critical revision of the manuscript for important intellectual content: SG and NM. Statistical analysis: SG, OA, DG, NM and AS-R.

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Shirley Gordon.

Ethics declarations

Conflict of interest

All the authors report no financial, personal or other relationships with commercial interests.

Additional information

Publisher’s Note

Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

Electronic supplementary material

Below is the link to the electronic supplementary material.

Supplementary material 1 (DOCX 27 KB)

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Gordon, S., Todder, D., Deutsch, I. et al. Effects of neurofeedback and working memory-combined training on executive functions in healthy young adults. Psychological Research 84, 1586–1609 (2020). https://doi.org/10.1007/s00426-019-01170-w

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s00426-019-01170-w