Skip to main content
Log in

Goodness takes effort: perceptual organization in dual-task settings

  • Original Article
  • Published:
Psychological Research Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

We adopted the Psychological Refractoriness Paradigm to study whether perceptual Goodness affects the load of central processing resources. In three dual-task experiments, 2-Alternative Forced Choice auditory classification was followed by a same–different task in which Garner’s classical five-dot patterns were presented. Goodness of these patterns and stimulus-onset asynchrony (SOA) between the first and second task were varied between trials. These factors had additive effects on response latencies and accuracy, indicating that pattern Goodness determines central processing load in performing the same–different task. In a fourth experiment, same–different judgment with Garner patterns was the single task. SOA between the first and second pattern was varied. Over-additive effects of Goodness with decreasing SOA were obtained, indicating sharing of central resources between consolidation of the first Garner pattern and performing the same–different task. Whereas, the resources needed for the latter, again, depend on Goodness, those needed for consolidation are independent of it.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Fig. 1
Fig. 2
Fig. 3
Fig. 4
Fig. 5

Similar content being viewed by others

Notes

  1. It was shown that in addition to response-selection, at least parts of the encoding stage require central capacity in certain conditions (Jolicoeur & Dell’ Acqua, 1998).

  2. Van Selst and Jolicoeur (1994) found under-additive interaction of mental rotation and SOA and concluded that at least some of the orientation-sensitive processes can operate in parallel with central processes of the first task.

  3. Only data entered the analysis of which RT met the following criterion: (RT < [μis+3σis|145 ms < RT< general speed criterion]) & (RT > [μ + 1.5σ|145 ms < RT < general speed criterion]), with μis = mean and σis = standard deviation of RT for the ith participant on the sth session and μ = mean and σ = standard deviation of RT, general speed criterion = 1,000 ms for the first task and 2,000 ms for the second task.

  4. For model fit, instead of β (R 2), we used the parameter R 2 adj = adjusted R 2 for low degree of freedom = R 2 − ([1 − R 2] a)/(ba*), with a = number of independent variables; b = sum of case weights; a* = number of coefficients (including intercept). R 2 adj is displayed when R 2 differs, i.e., when the significance is higher for R 2.

References

  • Bell, H. H., & Handel, S. (1976). The role of pattern Goodness in the reproduction of backward masked patterns. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 2, 139–150.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Berti, S., Geissler, H.-G., Lachmann, T., & Mecklinger, A. (2000). Event-related brain-potentials dissociate visual working memory processes under categorical and identical comparison conditions. Cognitive Brain Research, 9, 147–155.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Checkosky, S., & Whitlock, D. (1973). The effects of pattern Goodness on recognition time in a memory search task. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 100, 341–348.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Chipman, S., Mendelson, M. J., & Morton, J. (1979). Influence of six types of visual structure on complexity judgments in children and adults. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception & Performance, 5, 365–378.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Clement, D. E., & Varnadoe K. W. (1967). Pattern uncertainty and the discrimination of visual patterns. Perception and Psychophysics, 2, 427–431.

    Google Scholar 

  • Corballis, M. C., & Beale, I. L. (1970). Bilateral symmetry and behavior. Psychological Review, 77, 451–464.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Davis, R. (1959). The role of attention in the psychological refractory period. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 11, 211–220.

    Google Scholar 

  • De Jong, R. (1993). Multiple bottlenecks in overlapping task performance. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 19, 965–980.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Enns, J. T. (1987). A developmental look at pattern symmetry in perception and memory. Developmental Psychology, 23, 839–850.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Fagot, C., & Pashler, H. (1992). Making two responses to a single object: Implications for the central attentional bottleneck. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception & Performance, 18, 1058–1079.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Garner, W. R. (1962). Uncertainty and structure as psychological concepts. New York: Wiley.

    Google Scholar 

  • Garner, W. R. (1966). To perceive is to know. American Psychologist, 21, 11–19.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Garner, W. R., & Clement, D. E. (1963). Goodness of pattern and pattern uncertainty. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 2, 446–452.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Garner, W. R., & Sutliff, D. (1974). The effect of Goodness on encoding time in visual pattern discrimination. Perception and Psychophysics, 16, 426–430.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gottschaldt, K. (1926). Über den Einfluß der Erfahrung auf die Wahrnehmung von Figuren. Teil I: Über den Einfluß gehäufter Einprägung von Figuren auf ihre Sichtbarkeit in umfassenden Konfigurationen. [About the influence of experience to the perception of figures]. Psychologische Forschung/Psychological Research, 8, 261–283.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Heil, M., Rauch, M., & Hennighausen, E. (1998). Response preparation begins before mental rotation is finished: Evidence from event-related brain potentials. Acta Psychologica, 99, 217–232.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Heil, M., Wahl, K., & Herbst, M. (1999). Mental rotation, memory scanning, and the central bottleneck. Psychological Research, 62, 48–61.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Henderson, J. M. (1997). Transsacadic memory and integration during real-world object perception. Psychological Science, 8, 51–55.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Herbert, A. M., & Humphrey, G. K. (1996). Bilateral symmetry detection: Testing a “callosal” hypothesis. Perception, 25, 463–480.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Hogeboom, M. & van Leeuwen, C. (1997). Visual search strategy and perceptual organization covary with individual preference and structural complexity. Acta Psychologica, 95, 141–164.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Howe, E. S. (1980). Effects of partial symmetry, exposure time, and backward masking on judged Goodness and reproduction of visual patterns. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 32, 27–55.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Howe, E. S., & Brandau, C. J. (1980). The temporal course of visual pattern encoding: Effects of pattern Goodness. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Experimental Psychology, 35, 607–633.

    Google Scholar 

  • Irwin, D. E. (1991). Information integration across saccadic eye movements. Cognitive Psychology, 23, 420–456.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Jolicoeur, P., & Dell’ Acqua, R. (1998). The demonstration of short-term consolidation. Cognitive Psychology, 36, 138–202.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Kahneman, D. (1973). Attention and effort. New York: Prentice Hall.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kanizsa, G. (1985). Seeing and thinking. Acta Psychologica, 59, 23–33.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Kanizsa, G. (1994). Gestalt theory has been misinterpreted, but has also had some real conceptual difficulties. Philosophical Psychology, 7, 149–162.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Krueger, L. E. (1978). A theory of perceptual matching. Psychological Review, 85, 278–304.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Kubovy, M., & Wagemans, J. (1995). Grouping by proximity and multistability in dot lattices: A quantitative Gestalt theory. Psychological Science, 6, 225–234.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lachmann, T., & Geissler, H. G. (2002). Memory search instead of template matching? Memory-guided inference in same–different performance. Acta Psychologica, 111, 283–307.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Lachmann, T., & Geyer, T. (2003). Letter reversals in reading—Is the case really closed? A critical review and conclusions. Psychology Science, 45, 50–70.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lachmann, T., & van Leeuwen, C. (2004). Memory-guided inference in same–different comparison tasks. In C. Kaernbach, E. Schroeger, & H. J. Müller (Eds.), Psychophysics beyond sensation. Scientific psychology series (pp. 199–221). Hilldale, NJ: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lachmann, T., & van Leeuwen, C. (2005a). Individual pattern representations are context-independent, but their collective representation is context-dependent. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Experimental Psychology, 57 (in press).

  • Lachmann, T., & van Leeuwen, C. (2005b). Task-invariant Aspects of Goodness in Perceptual Representation. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Experimental Psychology, 57 (in press).

  • Logan, G. D., & Delheimer, J. A. (2001). Parallel memory retrieval in dual-task situations: II. Episodic memory. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 27, 668–685.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • McCann, R. S., & Johnston, J. C. (1992). Locus of the single-channel bottleneck in dual-task interference. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 18, 471–484.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Millspaugh, J. R. (1979). Effects of array organization on same–different judgments. Perception and Psychophysics, 23, 27–35.

    Google Scholar 

  • Navon, D., & Miller, J. (2002). Queuing or sharing? A critical evaluation of the single-bottleneck notion. Cognitive Psychology, 44, 193–251.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Palmer, S. E. (1985). The role of symmetry in shape perception. Acta Psychologica, 59, 67–90.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Pashler, H. (1984). Processing stages in overlapping tasks: Evidence for a central bottleneck. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception & Performance, 10, 358–377.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Pashler, H. (1989). Dissociations and dependencies between speed and accuracy: Evidence for a two-component theory of divided attention in simple tasks. Cognitive Psychology, 21, 469–514.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Pashler, H., & Johnston, J. C. (1989). Chronometric evidence for central postponement in temporally overlapping tasks. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 41, 19–45.

    Google Scholar 

  • Peterson, M. A., & Hochberg, J. (1983). Opposed-set measurement procedure: A quantitative analysis of the role of local cues and intention in form perception. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 9, 183–193.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Pomerantz, J. R. (1977). Pattern Goodness and speed of encoding. Memory and Cognition, 5, 235–241.

    Google Scholar 

  • Pomerantz, J. R., & Lockhead, G. R. (1994). Perception of structure: An overview. In G. R. Lockhead & J. R. Pomerantz (Eds.), The perception of structure (pp. 1–20). Washington, DC: American Psychological Association.

    Google Scholar 

  • Proctor, R. W. (1981). A unified theory for matching task phenomena. Psychological Review, 88, 291–326.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ruthruff, E., Miller, J. O., & Lachmann, T. (1995). Does mental rotation require central mechanisms? Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 21, 552–570.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Schubert, T. (1999). Processing differences between simple and choice reactions affect bottleneck localization in overlapping tasks. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 25, 408–425.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Schumacher, E. H., Seymour, T. L., Glass, J. M., Fencsik, D. E., Lauber, E. J., Kieras, D. E., et al. (2001). Virtually perfect time sharing in dual-task performance: uncorking the central cognitive bottleneck. Psychological Science, 12, 101–108.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Schweickert, R. (1978). A critical path generalization of the additive factor method: Analysis of a Stroop task. Journal of Mathematical Psychology, 18, 105–139.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Sebrechts, M. M., & Garner, W. R. (1981). Stimulus-specific processing consequences of pattern Goodness. Memory and Cognition, 9, 41–49.

    Google Scholar 

  • Shepard, R. N., & Metzler, J. (1971). Mental rotation of three-dimensional objects. Science, 171, 701–703.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Sternberg, S. (1966). High-speed scanning in human memory. Science, 153, 652–654.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Sternberg, S. (1969). The discovery of processing stages: Extensions of Donders’ method. Acta Psychologica, 30, 276–315.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Stins, J. F., & van Leeuwen, C. (1993). Context influence on the perception of figures as conditional upon perceptual organization strategies. Perception and Psychophysics, 53, 34–42.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Telford, C. W. (1931). The refractory phase of voluntary and associative responses. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 14, 1–36.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Tombu, M., & Jolicoeur, P. (2002). All-or-none bottleneck vs capacity sharing accounts for the psychological refractory period phenomenon. Psychological Research, 66, 274–286.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Van Leeuwen, C., & Van den Hof, M. (1991). What has happened to Praegnanz? Coding, stability, or resonance. Perception and Psychophysics, 50, 435–448.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Van Selst, M., & Jolicoeur, P. (1994). Can mental rotation occur before the dual-task bottleneck? Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception & Performance, 20, 905–921.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Van Lier, R., & Wagemans, J. (1997). Perceptual grouping measured by color assimilation: Regularity vs proximity. Acta Psychologica, 97, 37–70.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Van Selst, M., Ruthruff, E., & Johnston, J. C. (1999) Can practice eliminate the Psychological Refractory Period effect? Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception & Performance, 25, 1268–1283.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Vince, M. A. (1948). The intermittency of control movements and the psychological refractory period. British Journal of Psychology, 38, 149–157.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wagemans, J. (1993). Skewed symmetry: A nonaccidental property used to perceive visual forms. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 19, 364–380.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Welford, A. T. (1952). The “psychological refractory period” and the timing of high-speed performance—a review and a theory. British Journal of Psychology, 43, 2–19.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wertheimer, M. (1923). Untersuchungen zur Lehre der Gestalt II. [Investigation into Gestalt theory] Psychologische Forschung/Psychological Research, 4, 301–351.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wolfe, J. M., Cave, K. R., & Franzel, S. L. (1989). Guided search: An alternative to the feature integration model for visual search. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 15, 419–433.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgements

This work was supported by Grant La 1281/2-1 from the Deutsche Forschungsgemeinschaft (DFG) to Thomas Lachmann. Many thanks are due to Krisztin Pataki (TU Berlin), Annett Geiler, Kristin Neumann, and Karsten Zander (University of Leipzig) for technical support and to Peter Frensch, Mike Tombu and two anonymous reviewers for helpful comments on an earlier draft of this paper.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Thomas Lachmann.

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Cite this article

Lachmann, T., Leeuwen, C.v. Goodness takes effort: perceptual organization in dual-task settings. Psychological Research 71, 152–169 (2007). https://doi.org/10.1007/s00426-005-0001-y

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s00426-005-0001-y

Keywords

Navigation