Skip to main content
Log in

The effect of aborting ongoing movements on end point position estimation

  • Research Article
  • Published:
Experimental Brain Research Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

The present study investigated the impact of motor commands to abort ongoing movement on position estimation. Participants carried out visually guided reaching movements on a horizontal plane with their eyes open. By setting a mirror above their arm, however, they could not see the arm, only the start and target points. They estimated the position of their fingertip based solely on proprioception after their reaching movement was stopped before reaching the target. The participants stopped reaching as soon as they heard an auditory cue or were mechanically prevented from moving any further by an obstacle in their path. These reaching movements were carried out at two different speeds (fast or slow). It was assumed that additional motor commands to abort ongoing movement were required and that their magnitude was high, low, and zero, in the auditory-fast condition, the auditory-slow condition, and both the obstacle conditions, respectively. There were two main results. (1) When the participants voluntarily stopped a fast movement in response to the auditory cue (the auditory-fast condition), they showed more underestimates than in the other three conditions. This underestimate effect was positively related to movement velocity. (2) An inverted-U-shaped bias pattern as a function of movement distance was observed consistently, except in the auditory-fast condition. These findings indicate that voluntarily stopping fast ongoing movement created a negative bias in the position estimate, supporting the idea that additional motor commands or efforts to abort planned movement are involved with the position estimation system. In addition, spatially probabilistic inference and signal-dependent noise may explain the underestimate effect of aborting ongoing movement.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Fig. 1
Fig. 2
Fig. 3
Fig. 4
Fig. 5

Similar content being viewed by others

Notes

  1. The sudden movement abortion requirement implies high desired deceleration of the hand and short stopping time and distance. The CNS must specify motor commands, which correspond to force, to achieve this goal. In general, the stopping time and distance will emerge from the chosen motor commands and the current state of the system (i.e., position and speed) according to Newton’s second law of motion (F=ma). Therefore, the motor commands associated with movement termination are expected to be positively correlated with current movement speed.

References

  • Adamo DE, Martin BJ (2009) Position sense asymmetry. Exp Brain Res 192:87–95

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Adamo DE, Martin BJ, Brown SH (2007) Age-related differences in upper limb proprioception. Percep Mot skills 104:1297–1309

    Google Scholar 

  • Ambike S, Schmiedeler JP (2013) Invariant geometric characteristics of spatial arm motion. Exp Brain Res 229:113–124

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Ansems GE, Allen TJ, Proske U (2006) Position sense at the human forearm in the horizontal plane during loading and vibration of elbow muscles. J Physiol 576:445–455

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Ariff G, Donchin O, Nanayakkara T, Shadmehr R (2002) A real-time state predictor in motor control: study of saccadic eye movements during unseen reaching movements. J Neurosci 22:7721–7729

    PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Atkeson CG, Hollerbach AM (1985) Kinematic features of unrestrained vertical arm movements. J Neurosci 5:2318–2330

    PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Barnett CH, Harding D (1955) The activity of antagonist muscles during voluntary movement. Ann Phys Med 2:290–293

    PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Berardelli A, Hallett M, Rothwell JC, Agostino R, Manfredi R, Thompson PD, Marsden CD (1996) Single-joint rapid arm movements in normal subjects and in patients with motor disorders. Brain 119:661–674

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Bevan L, Cordo P, Carlton L, Carlton M (1994) Proprioceptive coordination of movement sequences: discrimination of joint angle versus angular distance. J Neurophysiol 71:1862–1872

    PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Brown SH, Cooke JD (1981) Amplitude and instruction-dependent modulation of movement-related electromyogram activity in humans. J Physiol 316:97–107

    PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Burke D, Hagbarth KE, Lofstedt L, Wallin BG (1976) The responses of human muscle spindle endings to vibration of non-contracting muscles. J Physiol 261:695–711

    PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Corcos DM, Gottlieb GL, Agarwal GC (1989) Organizing principles for single-joint movements II. A speed-sensitive strategy. J Neurophysiol 62:358–368

    PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Dassonville P (1995) Haptic localization and the internal representation of the hand in space. Exp Brain Res 106:434–448

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Davidson PR, Wolpert DM (2005) Widespread access to predictive models in the motor system: a short review. J Neural Eng 2:S313–S319

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Flanders M, Herrmann U (1992) Two components of muscle activation: scaling with the speed of arm movement. J Neurophysiol 67:931–943

    PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Fradet L, Lee G, Dounskaia N (2008) Origins of submovements during pointing movements. Acta Physiol 129:91–100

    Google Scholar 

  • Friedli WG, Hallett M, Simon SR (1984) Postural adjustments associated with rapid voluntary arm movements. I. Electromyographic data. J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry 47:611–622

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Gandevia SC, McCloskey DI (1977) Sense of heaviness. Brain 100:345–354

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Gandevia SC, Smith JL, Matthew C, Proske U, Taylor JL (2006) Motor commands contribute to human position sense. J Physiol 571:3703–3710

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Goble DJ, Noble BC, Brown SH (2010) Where was my arm again? Memory-based matching of proprioceptive targets is enhances by increased target presentation time. Neurosci Lett 481:54–58

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Goodwin GM, McCloskey DI, Matthews PB (1972) Proprioceptive illusions induced by muscle vibration: contribution by muscle spindles to perception? Science 24:1382–1384

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Gottlieb GL, Corcos DM, Agarwal GC (1989) Organizing principles for single-joint movements I. A speed-insensitive strategy. J Neurophysiol 62:342–357

    PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Gregory JE, Morgan DL, Proske U (1988) Aftereffects in the responses of cat muscle spindles and errors of limb position sense in man. J Neurophysiol 59:1220–1230

    PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Gritsenko V, Krouchev NI, Kalaska JF (2007) Afferent input, efference copy, signal noise, and biases in perceptions of joint angle during active versus passive elbow movements. J Neurophysiol 98:1140–1154

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Hagbarth KE, Nordin M, Bongiovanni LG (1995) After-effects on stiffness and stretch reflexes of human finger flexor muscles attributed to muscle thixotropy. J Physiol 482:215–223

    PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Itaguchi Y, Fukuzawa K (2012a) The influence of the indicator arm on end point distribution in proprioceptive localization with multi-joint arms. Exp Brain Res 222:77–88

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Itaguchi Y, Fukuzawa K (2012b) Effects of arm stiffness and muscle effort on position reproduction error in the horizontal plane. Percept Mot Skills 114:757–773

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Kawato M (1999) Internal models for motor control and trajectory planning. Curr Opin Biol 9:718–727

    CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Kording KP, Wolpert DM (2004) Bayesian integration in sensorimotor learning. Nature 427:244–247

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Laufer Y, Hocherman S, Dickstein R (2001) Accuracy of reproducing hand position when using active compared with passive movement. Physiother Res Int 6:65–75

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Lonn J, Djupsjobacka M, Johansson H (2001) Replication and discrimination of limb movement velocity. Somatosens Mot Res 18:76–82

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Marsden CD (1983) The function of the antagonist muscle during fast limb movements in man. J Physiol 335:1–13

    PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • McCloskey DI, Ebeling P, Goodwin GM (1974) Estimation of weights and tensions and apparent involvement of a “sense of effort”. Exp Neurol 42:220–232

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Morasso P (1981) Spatial control of arm movements. Exp Brain Res 42:223–227

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Proske U, Morgan DL, Gregory JE (1993) Thixotropy in skeletal muscle and in muscle spindles: a review. Prog Neurobiol 41:705–721

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Ribot-Ciscar E, Roll JP (1998) Ago-antagonist muscle spindle inputs contribute together to joint movement coding in man. Brain Res 791:167–176

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Roll JP, Vedel JP (1982) Kinaesthetic role of muscle afferents in man, studied by tendon vibration and microneurography. Exp Brain Res 47:177–190

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Schmidt RA, Zelaznik H, Hawkins B, Frank JS, Quinn JT Jr (1979) Motor-output variability: a theory for the accuracy of rapid motor acts. Psychol Rev 86:415–451

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Shergill SS, Bays PM, Frith CD, Wolpert DM (2003) Two eyes for an eye: the neuroscience of force escalation. Science 301:187

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Smith JL, Crawford M, Proske U, Taylor JL, Gandevia SC (2009) Signals of motor command bias joint position sense in the presence of feedback from proprioceptors. J Appl Physiol 106:950–958

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Soechting JF, Flanders M (1989) Sensorimotor representation for pointing to targets in three-dimensional space. J Neurophysiol 62:582–594

    PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Sperry RW (1950) Neural basis of the spontaneous optokinetic response produced by visual inversion. J Comp Physiol Psychol 43:482–489

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Suzuki M, Shiller DM, Gribble PL, Ostry DJ (2001) Relationship between cocontraction, movement kinematics and phasic muscle activity in single-joint arm movement. Brain Res 140:171–181

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Vallbo AB (1974) Afferent discharge from human muscle spindles in noncontracting muscles. Acta Physiol Scand 90:303–318

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • van Beers RJ, Sittig AC, van der Gon JJD (1998) The precision of proprioceptive position sense. Exp Brain Res 122:367–377

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • von Holst E (1954) Relations between the central nervous system and the peripheral organs. Brit J Anim Behav 2:89–94

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Walsh LD, Allen TJ, Gandevia SC, Proske U (2006) Effect of eccentric exercise on position sense at the human forearm in different postures. J App Physiol 100:1109–1116

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Waters P, Strick PL (1981) Influence of ‘strategy’ on muscle activity during ballistic movements. Brain Res 207:189–194

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Wilson ET, Wong J, Gribble PL (2010) Mapping proprioception across a 2D horizontal workspace. PLoS one 5:e11851

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Winter JA, Allen TJ, Proske U (2005) Muscle spindle signals combine with the sense of effort to indicate limb position. J Physiol 568:1035–1046

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Wolpert DM (2007) Probabilistic models in human sensorimotor control. Hum Mov Sci 26:511–524

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Wolpert DM, Flanagan JR (2001) Motor prediction. Curr Biol 11:R729–R732

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Wolpert D, Ghahramani Z, Jordan M (1995) An internal model for sensorimotor integration. Science 269:1880–1882

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Yoshihiro Itaguchi.

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Cite this article

Itaguchi, Y., Fukuzawa, K. The effect of aborting ongoing movements on end point position estimation. Exp Brain Res 231, 341–350 (2013). https://doi.org/10.1007/s00221-013-3697-8

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s00221-013-3697-8

Keywords

Navigation