Skip to main content
Log in

Sexual orientation and performance on sexually dimorphic motor tasks

  • Published:
Archives of Sexual Behavior Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

Performance of homosexual and heterosexual men and women on two motor tasks which reliably demonstrate sex differences in opposite directions was examined. A Throw-to-Target Task and the Purdue Pegboard were administered to undergraduate students matched for age and program of study. A two-way ANOVA (Sex × Sexual Orientation) of the Throw data showed a significant interaction,F(1, 90) = 16.22,p ≤ 0.001, and a trend for an effect of sex,F(1, 90) = 3.72,p < 0.06. Heterosexual men outperformed heterosexual women, whereas gay men threw less accurately and lesbians tended to throw more accurately than their heterosexual counterparts. Differences in sports history or hand strength did not account for these effects. Analysis of the Pegboard scores found no interaction or main effect of sexual orientation, but the effect of sex was significant,F(1,63) = 7.01,p ≤ 0.02. Regardless of sexual orientation, women outperformed men and this difference remained significant even when a measure of finger size was partialed out. This study provides new evidence suggesting an association between sexual orientation and motor-performance profiles. As with cognitive tasks, the motor-performance profiles of homosexuals are composites of some male-typical and some female-typical abilities. To the extent that sociological factors have been controlled, the study suggests that both sexual orientation and motor/cognitive predispositions may have early biological contributions.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Similar content being viewed by others

References

  • Allen, L. S., and Gorski, R. A. (1992). Sexual orientation and the size of the anterior commissure in the human brain.Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S. 89: 7199–7202.

    Google Scholar 

  • Brand, T., Kroonen, J., Mos, J., and Slob, K. (1991). Adult partner preference and sexual behavior of male rats affected by perinatal endocrine manipulations.Horm. Behav. 25: 323–341.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Brand, T., and Slob, A. K. (1991). On the organization of partner preference behaviour in female Wistar rats.Physiol. Behav. 49: 459–555.

    Google Scholar 

  • Dittmann, R. W., Kappes, M. E., and Kappes, M. H. (1992). Sexual behavior in adolescent and adult females with congenital adrenal hyperplasia.Psychoneuroendocrinology 17: 153–170.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Gladue, B. A., Beatty, W. W., Larson, J., and Staton, R. D. (1990). Sexual orientation and spatial ability in men and women.Psychobiology 18: 101–108.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gladue, B. A., Green, R., and Hellman, R. E. (1984). Neuroendocrine response to estrogen and sexual orientation.Science 225: 1496–1499.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Goy, R. W., and McEwen, B. S. (1980).Sexual Differentiation of the Brain, MIT Press, London, U.K.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hamer, D., Hu, S., Magnuson, V. L., Hu, N., and Pattatucci, A. M. L. (1993). A linkage between DNA markers on the X chromosome and male sexual orientation.Science 261: 321–327.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Hall, J. A., and Kimura, D. (1992). Homosexuality, cognitive abilities and the organizational hypothesis.Soc. Neurosci. Abstr. (507.17). 18: 1210.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hendricks, S. E., Graber, B., and Rodriguez-Sierra, J. F. (1989). Neuroendocrine responses to exogenous estrogen: No difference between heterosexual and homosexual men.Psychoneuroendocrinology 14: 177–185.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Jardine, R., and Martin, N. G. (1983). Spatial ability and throwing accuracy.Behav. Genet. 13: 331–340.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Kimura, D. (1973). Manual activity during speaking-2. Left-handers.Neuropsychologia 11: 51–55.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Kimura, D. (1993).Neuromotor Mechanisms in Human Communication, Oxford, New York.

  • Kinsey, A. C., Pomeroy, W. B., and Martin, C. E. (1948).Sexual Behavior in the Human Male, Philadelphia, PA, W. B. Saunders.

    Google Scholar 

  • LeVay, S. (1991). A difference in hypothalamic structure between heterosexual and homosexual men.Science 253: 1034–1037.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Lunn, D., and Kimura, D. (1989). Spatial abilities in preschool-aged children (Research Bulletin No. 681). University of Western Ontario, Department of Psychology.

  • McCormick, C. M., and Witelson, S. F. (1990). Cognitive abilities in lesbians.Soc. Neurosci. Abstr. (509.10). 16: 2.

    Google Scholar 

  • Meany, M. J., and McEwen, B. S. (1986). Testosterone implants into the amygdala during the neonatal period masculinize the social play of juvenile female rats.Brain Res. 398: 324–328.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Money, J., Schwartz, M., and Lewis, V. G. (1984). Adult erotosexual status and fetal hormonal masculinization and demasculinization: 46, XX congenital virilizing adrenal hyperplasia & 46, XY androgen-insensitivity syndrome compared.Psychoneuroendocrinology 9: 405–414.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Peters, M., Servos, P., and Day, R. (1990). Marked sex differences on a fine motor skill task disappear when finger size is used as a covariate.J. Appl. Psychol. 75: 87–90.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Resnick, S. M., Berenbaum, S. A., Gottesman, I. I., and Bouchard, T. J. (1986). Early hormonal influences on cognitive functioning in congenital adrenal hyperplasia.Dev. Psychol. 22: 191–198.

    Google Scholar 

  • Sanders, G. (1992). Sexual orientation and cognitive function.Int. J. Psychol. Abstr. (SY068.4). 27: 333.

    Google Scholar 

  • Sanders, G., and Ross-Field, L. (1986a). Sexual orientation, cognitive abilities and cerebral asymmetry: A review and a hypothesis tested.Ital. J. Zool. 20: 459–470.

    Google Scholar 

  • Sanders, G., and Ross-Field, L. (1986b). Sexual orientation and visuospatial ability.Brain Cognit. 5: 280–290.

    Google Scholar 

  • Sanders, G., and Wright, M. (1993, June). Sexual orientation differences in targeted throwing and manual dexterity tasks. Poster presented at the 19th annual meeting of the International Academy for Sex Research.

  • Swaab, D. F., and Hofman, M. A. (1990). An enlarged suprachiasmatic nucleus in homosexual men.Brain Res. 537: 141–148.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Tiffin, T. (1968).Purdue Pegboard Examiner Manual, Science Research Associates, Chicago.

    Google Scholar 

  • Tuttle, G. E., and Pillard, R. C. (1991). Sexual orientation and cognitive abilities.Arch. Sex. Behav. 20: 307–318.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Watson, N. V., and Kimura, D. (1991). Nontrivial sex differences in throwing and intercepting: Relation to psychometrically-defined spatial functions.Pers. Indiv. Diff. 12: 375–385.

    Google Scholar 

  • Williams, C. L., Barnett, A. M., and Meck, W. H. (1990). Organizational effects of early gonadal secretions on sexual differentiation in spatial memory.Behav. Neurosci. 104: 84–97.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Willmott, M., and Brierley, H. (1984). Cognitive characteristics and homosexuality.Arch. Sex. Behav. 13: 311–319.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Additional information

This research was supported by a grant from the Medical Research Council, Ottawa, to D. Kimura.

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Cite this article

Hall, J.A.Y., Kimura, D. Sexual orientation and performance on sexually dimorphic motor tasks. Arch Sex Behav 24, 395–407 (1995). https://doi.org/10.1007/BF01541855

Download citation

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/BF01541855

Key words

Navigation